Wednesday, March 27, 2019

A genetic mutation 60k to 130k years ago increased the capacity of general working memory or phonological storage, final piece in the evolution of human executive reasoning ability, language, & culture

Manoochehri, Majid. 2019. “The Evolution of Memory Span: A Review of the Existing Evidence.” PsyArXiv. March 27. doi:10.31234/osf.io/zdsya

Abstract: Memory span in humans has been intensely studied for more than a century. In spite of the critical role of memory span in our cognitive system, which intensifies the importance of fundamental determinants of its evolution, few studies have investigated it by taking an evolutionary approach. Overall, we know hardly anything about the evolution of memory components. In the current study, I reviewed the experimental studies of memory span in humans and non-human animals and the evolutionary hypotheses.

---
Memory span refers to the ability of an individual to reproduce immediately, after one presentation, a series of discrete stimuli (e.g., digits, letters, and words) in their original order (Blankenship, 1938). Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909) has been known as one of the first researchers of memory span. He studied the number of trials that it took him to memorize sequences of nonsense syllables (Ebbinghaus, 1885). He found that the number of trials needed increased with the length of the sequences. He then commented that: “The question can be asked: What number of syllables can be correctly recited after only one reading? For me the number is usually seven” (p. 36). In the years after that, several studies of memory span with verity of methods, tasks, and materials have been done by others (for review see Blankenship, 1938; Dempster, 1981). Surprisingly, some parts of results, such as the average memory span of adults were almost always the same. In 1956, Miller in his article discussed this issue. He stipulated that: “Everybody knows that there is a finite span of immediate memory and that for a lot of different kinds of test materials this span is about seven items in length” (p. 11). Today Miller’s suggestion has been known as the “magical number seven” of Miller, which refers to the memory span of young English adults. However, further findings from other nations were also in accordance with these results, suggesting that a memory span of about seven items is a universal characteristic of human beings. In addition to this feature, memory span scores have been discussed to be invariable and resistant to Flynn effects (Gignac, 2015; Wechsler, 1939). Moreover, memory span scores do not show considerable sex differences (Lynn & Irwing, 2008).The question that arises here is which selection pressures or evolutionary events caused the current memory span of humans and its particular characteristics.

The memory span of non-human animals
Firstly, it should be considered that because of the particular difficulties of studying cognitive functions in non-human animals, designed tasks in most of the available studies are somehow easier than classical memory span tests in humans. They usually include recognition of items instead of imitating the sequences, or several hours practice before the main test.

Chimps and other primates
Few studies have investigated memory span in non-human animals. Chimpanzees have been known as one of our nearest primate relatives and also one of the smartest non-human animals. Therefore, it may be expected to find the largest memory span of non-humans in them. The existing evidence shows that chimpanzees have a memory span of about 5 items (Inoue & Matsuzawa, 2007; Kawai & Matsuzawa, 2000). The results of other primates are very close to that of chimpanzees. Studies in baboons reveal a memory span of about 4 to 5 items (Fagot & De Lillo, 2011). On the other hand, the results of studying two rhesus monkeys by Swartz et al. (1991) suggest a memory span of about 4 objects.

Non-primates
Studies of non-primates do not exhibit better performances. Herman et al. (2013) have suggested a memory span of about 4 to 5 items for bottlenose dolphins. Lately, Toyoshima et al. (2018) have stipulated that rats are able to remember 5 objects at once. By contrast, a previous similar work by Sugita et al. (2015) has argued that rats’ memory span is approximately 4 items. A memory span of 4 items has also been suggested for pigeons. Terrace (1993) has discussed that the amount of time it takes a pigeon to learn a 4-item list (i.e., 3-4 months) suggests that 4 items may approach the limit of the pigeon's memory span.
Taking all these data together, one may conclude that there is an increasing trend of memory spans with a gentle slope from other animals to us. Moreover, it also seems reasonable to argue that while the memory span of most of the mentioned animals is about 4 to 5 items, humans’ memory span is about 7 items, which implies a sudden increase. This can be interpreted in favor of Coolidge and Wynn's (2005) hypothesis. They proposed that a mutation increased the length of memory span in the relatively recent human past. No need to emphasize that the idea of an increase with a gentle slope or a sudden increase is based on only a few experiments of memory span in some non-human animals. Surely, more studies are needed to provide the exact pattern. Experimental studies are also essential to measure the memory span of the current hunter-gatherers societies, such as Hadza of Tanzania. There are several reasons for the importance of their memory span results. Namely, because they are still foragers and their lifestyle is very close to that of our hunter-gatherer ancestors. Accordingly, their memory span might be similar, too. In addition, they are not literate or numerate and do not live in information-based societies. Therefore, they are not affected as broadly as other societies by some elements such as advanced educational programs. Moreover, another question is whether they apply memory’s strategies similar to people from other societies or not.

Evolutionary discussions and hypotheses
Until today, few studies have discussed memory span from an evolutionary perspective. There are also scant studies that discussed the evolution of working memory (e.g., Carruthers, 2013). It should be noticed that there are considerable differences between the functions of memory span and working memory. Therefore, there might be different selection pressures on them. In the present study the primary focus is on memory span.
Given the question of why memory span of humans has such a limited capacity, MacGregor (1987) based on a mathematical discussion emphasized the importance of an efficient retrieval. He suggested that: “… in a memory system evolved for efficient retrieval, there is an upper effective limit to short-term memory” (p. 107). From his point of view, there is an upper effective limit for the number of items in memory span, and this limitation guarantees an efficient retrieval.
The evolution of memory span has been also discussed by Coolidge and Wynn (2005). Although they mainly focused on working memory, the phonological lop which refers to memory span (or verbal memory span), has also been argued in their study. The core suggestion of their article is that a genetic mutation affected neural networks approximately 60,000 to 130,000 years ago and increased the capacity of general working memory or phonological storage. In case of memory span, they stipulated that:” A relatively simple mutation that increased the length of phonological storage would ultimately affect general working-memory capacity and language” (p. 14). They emphasized that an enhancement of capacities occurred in the relatively recent human past, most likely after the first appearance of anatomically modern humans, and this development was the final piece in the evolution of human executive reasoning ability, language, and culture. From their point of view, the larger capacity is a necessary precondition for symbolic thought, which selective pressures contributed to the growth of it. They noted that an increase in memory span of pre-modern Homo sapiens would have allowed greater articulatory rehearsal, consequently allowing for automatic long-term storage, and the beginnings of introspection, self-reflection, and consciousness. They, provided some evidence to support the assumption of a genetic mutation, such as the beginnings of composite tool-making about 300,000 years ago, or an explosion of culture which began approximately 50,000 years ago.
    In another part of their article, Coolidge and Wynn (2005) have quoted from Alan Baddeley that: “the phonological store evolved principally for the demands and acquisition of language” (p. 9). In addition, they have proposed that, evolutionary, the visuospatial sketchpad which is a part of working memory and maintains visual and spatial information may be older than the phonological loop. It should be noticed that the visuospatial sketchpad refers to visual memory span.

Conclusions
In the first place, the present article draws attention to the gap of evolutionary studies of memory span. It also suggests an increasing trend of memory span from our ancestors to us, whether the trend includes a steady increase with a gentle slope or a sudden increase. In terms of evolutionary discussions, the few available studies which have argued the evolution of memory span concentrated on some particular issues such as the reasons of a limited-capacity construct, or the results of increasing the length on cognitive functions. Other aspects remain untouched.

Latent genetic traits account for, on average, 40.63% of variance in traditional and online political talk, discussion with agreement and disagreement, and political conflict avoidance: Results from two twin studies

Genetic influence on political discussion: Results from two twin studies. Chance York. Communication Monographs, Mar 26 2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2019.1597274

ABSTRACT: Two twin studies are used to explore genetic influence on political discussion. Results from both studies demonstrate latent genetic traits account for, on average, 40.63% of variance in traditional and online political talk, discussion with agreement and disagreement, and political conflict avoidance. Taken together, the findings suggest a heritable genetic mechanism may partly explain why individuals vary across multiple dimensions of political discussion and differentially experience discussion effects. Implications for the political discussion effects literature and for reconceptualizing the etiology of political discussion are discussed.

KEYWORDS: Political discussion, individual differences, behavior genetics, twin study, genes

---
Political discussion is an important and beneficial form of communication in representa-tive democracies. Citizens who frequently discuss politics become more knowledgeableabout current events and public affairs, engaged civically and politically, and confidentin their own ability to affect their national government and its institutions (Eveland &Hively,2009; Kenski & Stroud,2006; Shah et al.,2017). Yet, citizens differ widely in behav-ioral dimensions of political discussion and thus any potential benefits they may derivefrom talking about politics. For instance, some citizens routinely discuss politics whileothers only talk about politics during salient election periods (Hardy & Scheufele,2009)if they engage in political discussion at all. Some citizens prefer face-to-face talk whileothers prefer online conversation and some citizens seek political argument whileothers seek agreement.
What accounts for these individual differences in political discussion? In the past, indi-vidual differences in political discussion have been thought to emerge from, and be con-strained by, personality characteristics (Gerber, Huber, Doherty, & Dowling,2012;Hibbing, Ritchie, & Anderson,2011), parent and peer socialization (Hively & Eveland,2009; Klofstad,2009, 2010, 2015), and a wide array of psychological motivations as out-lined in the Uses and Gratifications (U&G) theory of communication behavior(Eveland, Morey, & Hutchens,2011; Gil de Zúñiga, Valenzuela, & Weeks,2016;Kearney,2017). However, it is also plausible that far more fundamental traits–genes–precede and delimit these more immediate influences on discussion. Because genes arecentral to neuroanatomical development and neurochemical responses to external stimuli (Hatemi, Byrne, & McDermott,2012; Knopik, Neiderhiser, DeFries, & Plomin,2016; Lockyer & Hatemi,2018), they may indirectly promote pathways to explicit behav-ioral patterns, including patterns of interpersonal political communication. In addition,genes have been shown to regulate personality characteristics amenable to political talk,such as extraversion (Bouchard & Loehlin,2001). This is perhaps why genetic traitshave already been shown to partially explain orientations to interpersonal discussion gen-erally (Kirzinger, Weber, & Johnson,2012) and frequency of political discussion specifi-cally (Funk et al.,2010), though it remains unclear whether genetic traits also influenceother critical dimensions of political talk, including online political talk and discussionwith disagreement.
The purpose of this article is to therefore explore genetic influence on previously unex-amined aspects of political discussion. In this article, a behavior genetics perspective andtwin study survey data were used to ascertain the amount of variance in several dimen-sions of political discussion that can be attributed to latent genetic traits and the socialenvironment. Building on previous research (Funk et al.,2010; Kirzinger et al.,2012),the results show that variation in multiple dimensions of political discussion is partlyexplained by genetics. Importantly, the results suggest genes may be an overlookedfactor motivating behavioral differences in political discussion and its effects. Moreover,the results imply a need to reconceptualize the etiology of political discussion as beingshaped by both social-environmental and biological forces.

Literature review

Talking about politics is associated with a number of desirable effects. For example, pol-itical discussion is related to enhanced knowledge about political candidates and issues(Eveland,2004; Eveland & Hively,2009), more frequent engagement in civic and electoralprocesses (Bakker & De Vreese,2011; Klofstad,2009, 2010, 2015) heightened confidenceone can make a difference politically (Kenski & Stroud,2006), and increased tolerance forindividuals with differing political opinions (Mutz,2002a). Political talk also acts as a keymediator of news media influence, altering selection and processing of news content(Anspach,2017) and augmenting positive effects of news exposure on civic and politicalparticipation by enabling citizens to make sense of mediated information (Cho et al.,2009;Shah et al.,2007, 2017; Shah, Cho, Eveland, & Kwak,2005). Although“cross-cutting”dis-cussion with un-likeminded partisans has been linked to reduced levels of participationwithin politically diverse social networks (Mutz,2002b, 2006), a considerable body of evi-dence has shown political discussion to be cognitively and behaviorally advantageous.
Individuals vary, however, in how they discuss politics and how frequently, and thusany informational and participatory benefits they may reap from discussion. Forexample, while some citizens are consistent political discussants regardless of external cir-cumstances, others might be characterized as“seasonal discussants”who are“uniformedand unengaged and usually do not participate in political discussions”unless they are“triggered by highly salient campaign coverage” (Hardy & Scheufele,2009, pp. 95–97).Still other citizens may avoid political talk altogether despite exposure to externalevents, such as national elections, and despite possessing a general interest in politics.Some citizens may prefer interactions that take place online while others seek out face-to-face conversation. Some may enjoy political discussion with disagreement whileothers abandon conflict in pursuit offinding common ground on the issues.
As with other forms of communication, several factors can motivate individual differences in political discussion. To date, in fact, the literature provides at least three sourcesof variation. First, political talk may arise from and be bounded by deeply ingrained personality characteristics such as extraversion. Extraversion, which can be defined as an“energetic approach to the social and material world”(Gerber, Huber, Doherty, &Dowling,2011, p. 267), has been linked to more frequent political discussion with friends, family, and distant social ties (Hibbing et al.,2011) as well as more frequent dis-cussions that involve disagreement (Gerber et al.,2012). Personality traits such as agree-ableness, conscientiousness, openness to experience, and emotional stability have alsobeen associated with political discussion frequency and discussion with disagreement(Gerber et al.,2012; Hibbing et al.,2011). From this perspective,“psychological predis-positions captured by individual personality traits play an important role in shaping thekinds of conversations citizens engage in, the setting for those conversations, and theinfluence discussion may or may not have on the individual”(Hibbing et al.,2011,p. 602).
Second, individual differences in political discussion may result from socialization viapolitical interactions with parents and peers that take place during childhood, adolescence,and young adulthood. Youth who belong to“concept-oriented”families that encouragekids to share their ideas, for instance, tend to engage in more frequent, more informedand“elaborative”talk with their families (Hively & Eveland,2009) that may, in turn,shape how they discuss politics in the future. Similarly, discussing politics with collegeroommates has been linked to increased short-term civic and political participation (Klof-stad,2009) and long-term discussion habits (Klofstad,2010; 2015). An individual’s earlyexposure to political talk, especially discussions with parents and peers, may thus play amajor role in discussion patterns over the life course.A third source of variation in political discussion comes from the U&G theory of com-munication behavior. Specifically, U&G suggests that individuals are active communica-tors who engage in interpersonal discussion in a manner that is“goal-directed,purposive, and motivated”by deliberate choices to satisfy immediate psychologicalneeds to communicate affection, promote feelings of social inclusion, escape from one’sday-to-day routines, and relax with close social ties (Rubin,2009a, p. 167; see Barbato,Graham, & Perse,1997, 2003; Graham, Barbato, & Perse,1993; Rubin,2009b; Rubin,Perse, & Barbato,1988).
Politically oriented discussions may likewise satisfy needs to per-suade politically, express political opinions, enhance issue learning (Eveland et al.,2011;Gil de Zúñiga et al.,2016; Yoo, Kim, & Gil de Zúñiga,2017), as well as maintain one’spolitical identity and feel emotionally engaged via political interaction (Kearney,2017;Valenzuela & Bachmann,2015). Accordingly, the U&G theoretical framework suggeststhat individual differences in political discussion occur because people perceive discussionto satisfy differing informational and social needs.
As Sherry (2001) has noted, however, it is likely that individual differences in U&G needs and the communication behaviors they inspire originate in biological factors. Forinstance, while it has been suggested that U&G needs for communication are governed by psychological“ predispositions [and] the environment”(Rubin,2009a, p. 167), such“predispositions”like those rooted in personality traits are known to have an even more fundamental genetic basis (e.g., Bouchard & Loehlin,2001; Plomin, DeFries, Knopik,& Neiderhiser,2016). Additionally, it is established that because genes form the basis of human neuroanatomical development and control neurochemical responses to external stimuli, they ultimately provide pathways to explicit cognitive and behavioral patterns(Hatemi et al.,2012; Lockyer & Hatemi, 2018), including neural pathways that, alongwith social contexts, prompt and structure communication behavior (Beatty, McCroskey, & Pence,2009; Weber, Sherry, & Mathiak, 2009). It is therefore possible, as decades of twin research in the behavior genetics paradigm has shown, “that many physical, psychological,and behavioral traits, however indirectly, are the ultimate result of a combination ofgenetic inheritance and the environment” (Medland & Hatemi,2009, p. 192).

A behavior genetics approach to communication behavior
Behavior genetics researchers rely on known degrees of biological relatedness among survey respondents–typically, identical and fraternal twins–to estimate the impactof genes on observable outcomes. These outcomes have included highly specializedpsychological orientations and behaviors, such as political ideology, political party affilia-tion, and voter turnout (Hatemi et al.,2014; Hatemi & McDermott,2012; Hatemi,Medland, Morley, Heath, & Martin,2007); social and economic attitudes (Hatemi,Smith, Alford, Martin, & Hibbing,2015); educational attainment (Cesarini & Visscher,2017); conservativism, authoritarianism, and religiousness (Ludeke, Johnson, & Bouchard,2013); fear of social and political out-groups (Hatemi, McDermott, Eaves,Kendler, & Neale,2013), risk-taking behavior (Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman, & LaForge, 2005); and social cooperation and trust (Cesarini et al.,2008). Twin studies have also shown that genes play a powerful role in shaping dimensions of mental health (Strachan,Duncan, Horn, & Turkheimer,2017), personality (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Plominet al.,2016), and a variety of physical attributes such as height, weight, heart function,and facial structure (Polderman et al.,2015).1
In communication research, twin studies have demonstrated that genes partially explain differences in broad traits and orientations such as communicator style (Horvath,1995) and communicative adaptability (Beatty, Marshall, & Rudd,2001).
Twin studies have also been used to explore genetic influences on media consumptionand interpersonal communication behaviors. For example, one twin study demonstratedthat individual variation in mobile phone use, frequency of talking on mobile phones, andfrequency of texting were all indirectly influenced by genetic traits (Miller, Zhu, Wright,Hansell, & Martin,2012). Similar studies have shown that latent genetic traits influence frequency of using social media sites like Facebook to communicate with friends and family (York,2017), problematic internet use or PIU (Deryakulu & Ursavaş, 2014), and television viewing (Plomin, Corley, DeFries, & Fulker,1990).2
Across twin studies of communication, genetic traits have been shown to account forlarge proportions of individual variance in outcomes. For example, Kirzinger et al.(2012) showed genetic traits explained between 9% and 29% of the variance in orientationsto interpersonal discussion measured using two Likert-type items that tapped engagement in discussion and self-reported enjoyment in talking with“a lot of different people atparties”(p. 164). Funk et al. (2010, pp. 31–32) showed that up to a third of variance intwo Likert-type items measuring frequency of political discussion and conflict avoidance was explained by genes. These latter twin studies, combined with research that has foundthat genes provide brain–behavior pathways (Hatemi et al.,2012; Lockyer & Hatemi,2018) that activate patterns of communication (Beatty et al.,2009; Weber et al.,2009),suggest that additional dimensions of political talk may be partly explained by genetic traits. Thus, one could expect genetic traits to explain a nontrivial amount of variance in multiple dimensions of political talk, including traditional political discussion, online political discussion, political discussion with agreement and disagreement, and political conflict avoidance.In addition to examining whether genetic traits explain differences in dimensions of political discussion, it is also instructive in twin research to address the relative explana-tory contribution of genetic traits and the social environment shared by both twins (e.g.,being raised by the same parents) to the behaviors under investigation. Based on previousresearch, especially that of Kirzinger et al. (2012) and Funk et al. (2010), it may be the casethat both genetic and environmental factors shared by twins in a pair account for individ-ual differences in dimensions of political discussion, though it is uncertain which of thesefactors would make a more substantial contribution. Therefore, a research question is alsoposed:What is the relative explanatory contribution of genetic and environmental factors tovariance in multiple dimensions of political talk, including traditional political discussion,online political discussion, political discussion with agreement and disagreement, and pol-itical conflict avoidance?

Political journalists overwhelmingly interact with other journalists, particularly political ones, & their offline tendencies to form homogenous networks have transferred online

Exploring Political Journalism Homophily on Twitter: A Comparative Analysis of US and UK Elections in 2016 and 2017. Kelly Fincham. Media and Communication, Vol 7, No 1 (2019). March 21 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.17645/mac.v7i1.1765

Abstract: The tendency of political journalists to form insular groups or packs, chasing the same angles and quoting the same sources, is a well-documented issue in journalism studies and has long been criticized for its role in groupthink and homogenous news coverage. This groupthink attracted renewed criticism after the unexpected victory of Republican candidate Donald Trump in the 2016 US presidential election as the campaign coverage had indicated a likely win by the Democratic candidate Hillary Clinton. This pattern was repeated in the 2017 UK election when the Conservative party lost their majority after a campaign in which the news coverage had pointed to an overall Tory victory. Such groupthink is often attributed to homophily, the tendency of individuals to interact with those most like them, and while homophily in the legacy media system is well-studied, there is little research around homophily in the hybrid media system, even as social media platforms like Twitter facilitate the development—and analysis—of virtual political journalism packs. This study, which compares Twitter interactions among US and UK political reporters in the 2016 and 2017 national elections, shows that political journalists are overwhelmingly more likely to use Twitter to interact with other journalists, particularly political journalists, and that their offline tendencies to form homogenous networks have transferred online. There are some exceptions around factors such as gender, news organizations and types of news organization—and important distinctions between types of interactions—but overall the study provides evidence of sustained homophily as journalists continue to normalize Twitter.

Keywords: elections; groupthink; homophily; political journalism; Twitter, UK; US

Check also Journalistic Homophily on Social Media: Exploring journalists’ interactions with each other on Twitter. Folker Hanusch & Daniel Nölleke. Digital Journalism, https://www.bipartisanalliance.com/2018/02/journalists-continue-to-live-in-bubbles.html

---
The worst about this is not their constant sanctimoniousness when talking to the peasants, when they are bigger offenders and keep their membership in their echo chambers and sub-echo chambers in greater proportion that the great unwashed... It is that in journalism schools they are already told to be aware of their biases, and the very human need to be part of a pack (or herd?) and they forget it immediately after taking the exams.


---
2. Literature Review
FromthetelegraphtotypewriterstotelevisiontoTwitter, successive technological innovations have transformed the norms and practice of journalism (Lasorsa, Lewis, & Holton, 2012) and each new technology has arrived amid much fanfare about its potential impact on political communication, particularly around election campaigns (Stromer-Galley, 2014). Ultimately however, the expectations and concerns about these potential utopias and dystopias have never been fully realized as the power structures of journalism and politics have instead normalized each new “new media” into their own practice (Singer, 2005). The potential power of digital media in election campaigns was first seen in the US in the 2004 Presidential campaign when it rocketed the relatively unknown candidate Howard Dean into the political and media stratosphere (Stromer-Galley, 2014) but as Margolis and Resnick had already argued in 2000, any of the digital advantages accruing to early adopters like Dean were soon eclipsed as the political and journalism elite folded these new technologies into existing practices when they recognized,a nd there by normalized, the “new” new media (Margolis & Resnick, 2000). Much of the research into Twitter journalism practice argues that journalists, seen as frequent, if not always skillful, Twitter users (Engesser & Humprecht, 2015) are well down the path of normalization, using Twitter in ways that conform to existing practice rather than using it to change journalism practice (see Lasorsa et al., 2012; Lawrence, Molyneux, Coddington, & Holton,2014; Lewis,2012; Molyneux & Mourão,2019; Nuernbergk,2016;Parmelee,2013). This is especially evidentinare as such as gatekeeping,wherejournalistshave long controlled whose voices make it through the editorial “gates” (Lasorsa et al., 2012; Singer, 2005), and Twitter gatekeeping can be seen in the “insider talk” and “regurgitation” of information flowing across Twitter (Lawrence et al., 2014; Parmelee, Roman, Beasley, & Perkins, 2019, p. 161) as journalists more frequently engage with other journalists or newsmakers—and even themselves—rather than interest groups, academics or citizens (Carlson, 2017; Molyneux & Mourão, 2019). While journalists can, and do, challenge normalization in other areas of journalism practice (see Broersma & Graham 2016; Molyneux & Mourão, 2019), this study’s sole concern is whether political journalists create homogenous packs on Twitter, thus supporting the idea of homophily, and by extension, normalization, even as the hybrid media system (Chadwick, 2013) theoretically presents alternatives to the pack model with a wider range of interaction partners and voices outside the bubbles. While some studies indicate more negotiation around normalization in newer affordances such as quote tweetsor areas such as monitoring, sourcing, publishing, promoting and branding (Broersma & Graham, 2016; Molyneux & Mourão, 2019; Tandoc & Vos, 2016), the research overwhelmingly indicates that journalists’ interactions are dominated by other journalists andthat these homogenous onlinenetworks resemblethosebuilt by journalists offline (Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018). However, despite the plethora of studies indicating that journalists’ Twitter networks are so homogenous as to suggest homophily there has been little research so far specifically into homophily in those interactions even as journalists themselves report low levels of citizen engagement. For example, Gulyas (2017) found journalist/ citizen interaction at 23 and 27 percent in the US and UK respectively, and Nuernbergk (2016) saw only rare interactions between German journalists and their Twitter followers, thus suggesting that political journalists still prefer to connect with each other in “journalism-centered bubbles” (Molyneux & Mourão, 2019; Mourão, 2015; Nuernbergk, 2016, p. 877). Additionally, researchers have noted evidence of bubbles within bubbles (Bentivegna & Marchetti, 2018) with political journalists seen as more likely to interact with other political journalists (Hanusch & Nölleke,2018); selfsegregating by gender (Artwick, 2013; Usheretal.,2018), and focusing on those inside their own news organization (Bentivegna & Marchetti, 2018; Larsson, Kalsnes, & Christensen, 2017) with Vergeer (2015) reporting that regional reporters were more likely to do this than national journalists. While these studies were broad in nature, Hanusch and Nölleke (2018) specifically considered thepotential impactofbeat, gender,organizational context and geographic proximity in an extensive inquiry into homophily among Australian reporters and found a high degree of homophily across those four sharedcharacteristics. Homophily, or the tendency of individuals to form groups with those most similar to themselves (McPhersonetal.,2001)wasintroduced as a concept in the 1950s when Lazarsfeld and Merton (1954) proposed that individuals were far more likely to build networks around shared values in areas like religion or sport or around shared status in areas such as race, ethnicity, sex, age, religion, education and occupation (Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018; McPherson et al., 2001). As an elite specialty within the wider occupational field of journalism, political journalists are perhaps more sensitive to the homophilous effects of these tight-knitgroups as they seek validation from “those to whom we compare ourselves, those whose opinions we attend to, and simply those whom weare aware of andwatchforsignals aboutwhat is happening in our environment” (McPherson et al., 2001, p. 428). The tendency for political reporters to focus on each other was first labelled as “pack journalism” during the 1972 US presidential election when Rolling Stone reporter Tim Crouse noted that the journalists’intentfocusoneacho therled to a shared groupthink about the day’s most important stories and created a pack dynamic so strong that “almost all the reporters will take the same approach to the story”, even though they were ostensibly competing against each other(Crouse,1973). As former Newsweek Bureau Chief Karl Fleming said: “Their (the reporters’) abiding interest is making sure that nobody else has got anything that they don’t have—not getting something that nobody else has”(Crouse,1973). While Crouse observed the political journalism network and the resulting groupthink from his physical seat on the campaign bus, researchers can now observe virtual political journalism networks from afar through the analysis of publicly-visible Twitter conversations and the use of affordances such as retweets, replies, mentions and followings. Retweet and mention networks (which include both replies and indirect mentions) are often seen as the strongest interaction markers (Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018) and several studies have reported differences in the way journalistsuseretweets and mentions with more homophily seen in mentions than retweets (Hanusch & Nölleke 2018; Molyneux & Mourão, 2019; Nuernbergk, 2016). However, indirect mentions can be also be used as a “shout out” (Usher et al., 2018) thus diluting their effectiveness as a distinct measure of interactive intent. Retweets, despite multiple Twitter disclaimers to the contrary (Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018), are most often viewed as an endorsement of content (Meraz & Papacharissi, 2013; Russell, Hendricks, Choi, & Stephens, 2015), but they also convey endorsement of the user and the link between the original and retweeting sender provides evidence of a pre-existing homophilous network of like-minded people (Bruns & Burgess, 2012; Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018). While some journalists use replies to thread longer posts together and circumvent Twitter’s 280-character count (Molyneux & Mourão, 2019, p. 257), specific replies (as against indirect mentions) are more typically interactive with some research indicating potential heterophily with studies showing “public/citizen” users receiving as high as 48 percent of the journalists’replies (Brems, Temmerman, Graham, & Broersma, 2017). However, these studies don’t mention if the accounts received more than one reply which would help us consider the nature and value of such interactions, a problem noted by Parmelee and Deeley in 2017, when they queried the use of simple counts arguing that such one-offs were inadequate ways to measure reciprocity. Such reciprocity is often absent in followings (Kiousis, 2002) and, as Ausserhoffer and Maireder reported in 2013, followings are not a reliable metricasthey can bepaid for or artificially enhanced by computer scripts. Subsequently, this study views the affordances of retweets and replies as more indicative of actual intent, highlighting the user’s value to the journalist (Conover et al.,2011; Molyneux, 2015). Frequency of interactions is also important. As McPherson et al .(2001) outlined, homophily can be seen in those whose “opinions we attend to” and given the concerns raised by Parmelee and Deeley (2017) around one-off replies, this study measures interactivity by focusing on the political journalists’ most-frequent discussion partners in replies and retweets to see which voices the journalists most frequently attend to. This research builds on the developing work into Twitter journalism homophily (see particularly Hanusch & Nölleke, 2018) and is important as it is the first to examine this issue in the context of social media election coverage, specifically on Twitter, and takes the analysis further by looking at media practice in two similar media systems. The importance of studies such as this, which examine these “new” types of interactions on social media, cannot be overstated as the work done by political journalists remains essential to a citizen’s ability to understand politics and election campaigns even in a digital and networked age (Harder, Paulussen, & VanAelst, 2016; Kuhn & Nielsen, 2014).


3.Research Questions
This study explores retweets and replies as two distinct affordances and explores them separately for the presence of homophily by asking the following two research questions:

RQ1: To what extent can homophily be identified in political journalists’ retweets on Twitterin an election campaign?
RQ2: To what extent can homophily be identified in political journalists’ replies on Twitter in an election campaign?

Drawing from the categories devised in Hanusch and Nölleke’s study (2018) the study then considers if organizational context, types of news organization or gender can be seen to play a role in homophily in political journalists’ retweets and replies, which leads to these research questions:

RQ3:Doshared characteristics such as news organizations; type of news organizations and gender play a role in homophily in retweets?
RQ4: Do shared characteristic ssuch as news organizations; type of news organizations and gender play a role in homophily in replies?

While Democrats show stronger negative emotional response to moral violations than Republicans, partisans of both parties express significantly greater negativity when a politician of the other party violates a moral foundation

Voters’ Partisan Responses to Politicians’ Immoral Behavior. Annemarie S. Walter, David P. Redlawsk. Political Psychology, March 27 2019. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12582

Abstract: Politicians’ moral behaviors affect how voters evaluate them. But existing empirical research on the effects of politicians’ violations of moral standards pays little attention to the heterogeneous moral foundations of voters in assessing responses to violations. It also pays little attention to the ways partisan preferences shape responses. We examine voters’ heterogeneous evaluative and emotional responses to presumably immoral behaviors by politicians. We make use of moral foundation theory’s argument that people vary in the extent to which they endorse, value, and use the five universally available moral intuitions: care, fairness, loyalty, authority and sanctity. We report on a 5 × 3 between‐subjects experiment asking a random sample of 2,026 U.S. respondents to respond to politicians’ violations of different moral foundations. We randomly vary which of the five foundations is violated and the partisanship of the actor (Republican/ Democrat /Nonpartisan). Results suggest that partisanship rather than moral foundations drives most of U.S. voters’ responses to moral foundations violations by politicians. These foundations seem malleable when partisan actors are involved. While Democrats in this sample show stronger negative emotional response to moral violations than Republicans, partisans of both parties express significantly greater negativity when a politician of the other party violates a moral foundation.


---
Immoral behavior by politicians is nothing new. The candidacy and subsequent election of Donald Trump to the U.S. presidency seems to suggest that in the current American political environment, moral violations may be more rule than exception. During his campaign, Trump was accused of sexual misconduct as a tape surfaced where he talked about “grabbing them by the pussy,” while a number of women came forward accusing Trump of inappropriate and sexually harassing behaviors that in other times would have doomed his candidacy. Trump routinely verbally violated a wide range of moral norms during his campaign, for example, mocking a reporter for his disabilities and referring to a former Miss Universe contestant campaigning for Hillary Clinton as “Miss Piggy.” Even after the election, Trump continued to violate basic moral positions that might have sunk other presidents. Recently, Stormy Daniels, a pornographic movie star, alleged that she had had an affair with Trump and he paid her to cover it up just before the November 2016 vote. Unlike John Edwards, the 2008 Democratic presidential candidate whose campaign was doomed when an affair came to light, Trump continues with little obvious impact on his favorability ratings.
Each of the above would be considered clear moral violations by moral foundation theorists, in particular, violations of the foundations of “care” and “sanctity” (Graham et al., 2011; Haidt & Graham, 2011). Among Trump’s most consistent supporters are religious conservative voters who generally perceive themselves as high in morality in everyday life, while identifying as strong Republicans when it comes to politics. This apparent conflict between moral values and partisan preferences suggests a need to examine the link between voters’ endorsements of moral foundations and their responses to moral transgressions of those foundations by politicians. If moral foundations are, in fact, innate and foundational, voter’s moral values should dominate when a foundation is violated. A voter who strongly cares about a particular moral foundation should react negatively to its violation, regardless of the party of the politician involved. And yet, as the Trump example makes clear, there is reason to question this belief. Over 90% of Republican voters supported Trump in 2016, despite his continual violations of moral foundations, and presumably in opposition to their own support of those same foundations.
We wish to examine the extent to which underlying moral values subscribed to by American voters condition responses to violations of moral foundations by politicians. We consider whether the extent to which people care about moral foundations like care, fairness, loyalty, authority, and sanctity influences their negative emotional responses to violations. Alternatively, given the partisan nature of American politics in the early twenty‐first century, it may be that partisan agreement is more important than moral foundations. We seek to answer the question of whether partisanship in America also extends to the point of overriding, or at least reducing, the effect of underlying moral values.
Partisanship is a core feature of the American political system. It acts as a perceptual screen (Campbell, Converse, Miller, & Stokes, 1960) coloring how partisans view all aspects of politics. More recent research on motivated reasoning (Lodge & Taber, 2013; Redlawsk, 2002; Taber & Lodge, 2006) provides a mechanism for this process, as existing affective evaluations—such as partisan preference—influence the cognitive processing of relevant information. As a result, in an era of increasing partisan and social sorting, partisan preference may provide a great deal of cover for politicians who violate moral foundations. Mason (2018) documents how American social and political identities have recently aligned so that previous crosscutting cleavages have all but disappeared. As a result, partisanship is now reinforced by other social identities, including religious identities. There may be good reason to think that moral foundations themselves can become subsumed within partisan identity, so that violations of foundations by the “other side” are much worse than violations by “our side.”
At the same time, studies have shown that exposure to scandals depresses voters’ candidate evaluations (Bhatti, Hansen, & Olsen, 2013; Carlson, Ganiel, & Hyde, 2000; Doherty et al., 2011) and reduces trust in political institutions and the political process (Bowler & Karp, 2004; Maier, 2011). Politicians’ moral transgressions are extensively covered by the media (Fogarty, 2013). This does not appear to have changed in recent years; the allegations against Trump were certainly front and center in media reports during the 2016 presidential campaign. Moreover, following Trump’s election, allegations of sexual harassment against other powerful men in and out of politics spurred the #metoo movement, as women recounted their experiences. Former Sen. Al Franken, thought to be in the mix for the presidential campaign in 2020, was forced to resign, while others have also lost positions of power. Yet the very same kinds of claims against Trump did not, in the end, derail his candidacy, nor so far, his presidency.
Emotions play an important role in moral foundations theory (Haidt, 2003) and are key factors in voters’ moral judgments about politicians and institutions (Ben‐Nun Bloom, 2014; Bowler & Karp, 2004). Emotions not only often guide moral judgments, but also shape voting behavior. Emotions have been found to underpin political campaigns generally (Marcus et al., 2000; Redlawsk, 2006) as voters both think and feel about politics. Negative emotions can be especially important. Among the many aspects of politics that might trigger emotional responses, we would expect violations of moral foundations by politicians to be central, with voters expressing negative emotions about violators, but only to the extent that the voters themselves care about any given foundation that has been violated.
Despite the prominent role of emotions in explaining political behavior and numerous studies examining effects of politicians’ violations of moral standards, little attention has been paid to the intersection of the two, that is, how voters respond emotionally to politicians’ moral violations (the notable exceptions are Halmburger, Rothmund, Schulte, & Baumert, 2012 and Jiang et al., 2011). In addition, to our knowledge, no study has examined how heterogeneous preferences for moral foundations condition how voters respond to politicians’ moral transgressions.
This study thus aims to answer three research questions: (1) How do American voters respond emotionally to violations of moral foundations by politicians? (2) Are voters’ emotional responses conditioned by their own moral values? and (3) Does partisanship influence the negative emotional responses voters have to violations of moral foundations? To examine these questions, we conducted a 5 × 3 between‐subjects experiment with an online random sample of about 2,000 U.S. voters. We manipulated the moral foundation violated by a politician (care, fairness, loyalty, authority, and sanctity) and the partisanship of the politician involved (Republican, Democrat and no partisan label).
We find voters express negative emotional responses to politicians’ moral transgressions, but the level of negativity is strongly conditioned by partisanship. Democratic voters have stronger negative emotional responses to many of these moral violations than do Republicans. At the same time, partisans of both parties express more negative emotions when a politician of the other party violates moral foundations, all else equal, while responding more similarly to a nonpartisan actor. Finally, while we anticipated that a voter who endorses the values of a particular foundation to a greater degree would be more negative when it is violated, this effect was and clearly less than the effects of party when partisan actors were involved.
The remainder of this article proceeds as follows. First, we discuss moral foundation theory and the role of emotions. Second, we summarize the literature explaining individual variance in response to politicians’ immoral behaviors and develop hypotheses from this literature. Third, the experimental design, analysis strategy, and operationalization of the variables are discussed. Finally, results are presented and conclusions are drawn.

Moral Violations and Emotions

We build on two strands of literature: the scandal literature from political science and literature on (moral) emotions and moral political judgements from social psychology. Moral judgment is the evaluation of an act as morally wrong or right (Ben‐Nun Bloom, 2014). Moral transgressions, that is, harm to others’ welfare, are thought to be inherently wrong since they have an intrinsic effect on the well‐being of others (Ben‐Nun Bloom, 2014). Moral transgressions by politicians can become scandals, although the word “scandal” itself does not refer to the moral transgression, but to the communicative event surrounding the moral transgression becoming public (Lee, 2015).
Moral foundation theory (MFT) sees moral judgment as an intuitive process characterized by automatic affective reactions to stimuli (Clifford, Iyengar, Cabezzam, & Sinnott‐Armstrong, 2015). This is in line with the social intuitionism model of morality (Haidt, 2001) which argues that people know intuitively whether acts are right or wrong. They are capable of swift judgment of an (im)moral act, but they take considerably more time to come up with a rationale when asked to explain their judgment (Haidt & Hersh, 2001). Haidt and Hersh (2001) argue that intuitions and emotions most often precede and guide moral emotions.
MFT categorizes moral intuitions into five foundations: care, fairness, loyalty, authority, and sanctity (Haidt & Graham, 2011). Care refers to the dislike for the suffering of others; fairness to a commitment to fairness and justice. Loyalty is seen as a commitment to one’s own group. Authority refers to respect for authority and tradition, and sanctity refers to concerns with purity and contamination. People differ in the extent to which they endorse these five values, and thus MFT also provides an understanding of moral diversity (Graham et al., 2011). MFT extends most scales used in moral psychology as it does not limit the moral domain to concerns about individuals harming or unfairly treating other individuals (Graham et al., 2011). Moreover, MFT is meant to cover the full range of moral concerns, including those found in non‐Western cultures, in religious practices, and among political conservatives (Graham et al., 2011). Studies have found that political liberals and conservatives differ in the weight that they place on the various moral foundations (Graham et al., 2009; Haidt & Graham, 2007). Specifically, liberals have been found to rate considerations of care and fairness as significantly more important moral values than loyalty, authority, or purity. To liberals, acts are perceived as immoral primarily to the extent that they harm others or treat people unfairly.
There appear to be only two studies that have examined people’s emotional responses to moral transgressions by politicians (Halmburger et al., 2012; Jiang et al., 2011). Both studies report that exposure to a political scandal generates negative emotions towards the politician involved. Halmburger et al. (2012) incorporates specific moral emotions in their study, reporting higher levels of anger and shame when subjects are exposed to a news report including a politician’s moral transgression. They also find that negative moral emotions stimulate need for retribution versus need for restoration of the moral transgressing politician (Halmburger et al., 2012). But these studies are of limited generalizability since they do not effectively account for the role partisanship plays in conditioning responses when partisan actors are involved.
Although moral foundation theory is a prominent theory, it is not uncontroversial. Various scholars criticize the assumptions underlying MFT, such as the innateness and stability of moral foundations (Smith, Alford, Hibbing, Martin, & Hatemi, 2017), the existence of five or six distinct moral foundations underlying moral judgment (Schein & Gray, 2018), and the strength and direction of the relationship between moral foundations and political predispositions (Ciuk, 2018; Smith et al., 2017). Most recently, Connors (2019) reports that political values—like moral foundations thought by most scholars to be core beliefs—are readily influence by the social environment. Even with this, the theory is well enough established with key implications for politics that call for testing it in the political context we do here, following work by Clifford et al. (2015).

Heterogeneous Responses to Politicians’ Moral Transgressions

Moral transgressions by a politician should signal to voters that he or she is an immoral candidate, which should negatively affect the candidate’s electoral prospects. If it were that simple, we would have little to examine here: Voters would simply punish those who violate moral standards, with those feeling more strongly about a given moral foundation responding more negatively. However, politicians embroiled in scandals are not always electorally punished for their moral transgressions, and individual voters’ responses to such transgressions differ in strength (Fernández‐Vázquez, Barberá, & Rivero, 2016). This has puzzled scholars and stimulated research trying to understand the psychology of the public’s heterogeneous reactions to scandals (e.g., Fischle, 2000; Halmburger et al., 2012; Lee, 2015).
Numerous factors are mentioned as potential sources for this variance in voters’ responses. Voters may respond differently to different types of scandals (Bhatti et al., 2013; Carlson et al., 2000; Doherty et al., 2011; Fernández‐Vázquez et al., 2016). Thompson (2013) distinguishes three types of scandals, namely sex scandals, financial fraud scandals, and corruption scandals. Financial scandals are punished more severely than sex scandals (Brenton, 2011; Carlson et al., 2000, Funk, 1996), although Doherty et al. (2011) notes this holds only as long as the sex scandal does not involve abuse of power. The identity of the politician involved matters as does the politician’s response to the moral transgression (Lee, 2015; Tiedens, 2001). Gender appears related to voters’ judgments (Brenton, 2011), but probably in combination with the type of scandal (Carlson et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2015).
Other research has shown that trait impressions and prior affect for the politician influence voters’ responses (Fischle, 2000; Funk, 1996). In judging a politician’s moral transgression, Funk (1996) argues that perceived competence matters more than perceived warmth, but only for the more politically knowledgeable voters (Funk, 1996). Recently, Laustsen and Bor (2017) have shown in an electoral context that warmth is the most influential candidate trait on which people judge politicians, perhaps challenging Funk. It also matters how credible voters perceive the information about a scandal—especially when there are claims that the politician committed the transgression intentionally (Anduiza, Gallego, & Munoz, 2013; Lee, 2015). The relevance and importance of the transgression also influences voters’ responses (Anduiza et al., 2013; Lee, 2015). These perceptions are also affected by how their news sources and the media in general frame the scandal (Peterson & Vonnahme, 2014; Shah, Watts, Domke, & Fan, 2002).
Finally, and especially relevant for our study, political identity in the form of partisanship may influence voters’ perceptions of politicians’ immoral behavior (Anduiza et al., 2013; Bhatti et al., 2013; Blais et al., 2010; Fischle, 2000). Partisan preferences can engage motivated reasoning processes that lead voters to discount or otherwise accept behavior from politicians who share those preferences, that they would not for politicians from another party (Kunda, 1990; Redlawsk, 2002). People selectively process information in ways that enable them to arrive at conclusions congruent and congenial to their prior beliefs, including political beliefs (Fischle, 2000). This process can readily lead to partisans rejecting information about immoral behavior by a copartisan politician as not credible. Even when they acknowledge the moral transgression, partisan voters might still bear a less negative judgment about their party’s candidate. While partisanship may not affect perceptions of the facts of the scandal, it may still affect political judgment (Blais et al., 2010).
The usual assumption is that partisan bias works both ways, so partisans perceive their own party more positively and other parties more negatively. However, Blais, Gidengil, and Kilibarda (2017) argue that the partisan effect is asymmetrical, although they note there has been little systematic investigation of how symmetric (or asymmetric) it might be. They find that partisans view their own parties as less corrupt than do nonpartisans, but they do not necessarily view other parties as more corrupt. Anduiza et al. (2013) also find an asymmetrical effect, arguing that moral transgressions are judged differently by voters depending on whether the politician involved is a member of the respondent’s party, rival party, or of an unknown affiliation. However, not all studies find this partisan effect when it comes to how voters process politician’s moral violations (Halmburger et al., 2012). Some find that political sophistication interacts with this partisan bias, and the partisan bias is absent among the more politically sophisticated.

Hypotheses

Considering the prominence that MFT has gained in social psychology, it seems surprising that political scientists have not used it yet to try to explain voters’ responses to moral violations by politicians. Certainly, moral violations occur, and voters historically have seemed to care about them, even if responses might be tempered for one’s own party. While there is evidence that partisans on different sides of the aisle see different moral foundations as salient (Haidt & Graham, 2011), examining all five foundations should let us get a better understanding of how voters respond to their violations and in particular, the extent of negative emotions generated by violations. Thus, the literature we have reviewed above leads us to propose the following four hypotheses:
H1 (Partisanship and Negative Emotions Hypothesis): Across parties, respondents will have negative emotional responses to politicians committing moral violations, all else equal. But based on work by Haidt and Graham (2011), Democrats (typically liberals) will show stronger negative emotions in response to violations of care and fairness specifically, compared to Republicans. Given no prior evidence of partisan effects, we do not have specific expectations about partisan responses to the other three foundations: loyalty, authority, and sanctity.
H2 (Moral Values Hypothesis): Negative emotional responses to violations of moral foundations by politicians will be conditioned on voters’ own endorsements of particular moral values. The more that respondents endorse a particular moral value, the stronger their negative emotional response will be when a politician violates that particular moral foundation.
H3 (Partisanship Interaction Hypothesis): Partisan respondents will be less negative about violations of moral foundations by politicians of their own party, compared to violations by out‐party and nonpartisan politicians committing the same violation.
H4 (Moral Values by Partisanship Interaction Hypothesis): Moral foundations are thought to be based on innate, evolutionarily developed intuitive ethics, where “moral judgment is caused by quick moral intuitions” (Haidt, 2001, p. 817). At the same time, in politics, we know that partisanship acts in many ways as a perceptual screen (Campbell et al., 1960), conditioning how voters respond to partisan information. Thus, when partisanship is not invoked in a moral foundation violation, we expect the strength of a given moral value to drive emotional response to it. However, when the actor is a partisan and so is the voter, we expect that partisan preference will moderate these effects.
This leads to a testable hypothesis: Respondents will express a lower level of negativity toward co‐partisans violating a given foundation, compared to a nonpartisan or other party actor, at all levels of moral values strength. That is, even though voters with stronger moral values should be more negative to violations of the corresponding moral foundation (H2), partisan‐motivated reasoning should moderate these effects. However, for a nonpartisan actor, respondents who more strongly support a given moral value will be more negative about its violation than those for whom the value is less important.

Discussion and Conclusions

This study set out to answer three questions, namely (1) How do American voters respond emotionally to violations of moral foundations by politicians? (2) Are voters’ emotional responses conditioned by their own moral values? and (3) Does partisanship minimize the effects of violations of moral foundations by politicians of the voter’s own party compared to the other party? We find that in general voters respond with negative emotions to politicians’ moral violations. However, not all voters respond in the same manner; we find that Democrats tend to respond more negatively to this set of moral violations than do Republicans. We might speculate that the political environment in which our study was done could have played a role in this unexpected result. As we detailed at the beginning of the article, there have been many accusations of moral violations by President Trump, none of which seem to shake his core Republican supporters. One impact of this may have been to lessen Republican voters’ sensitivity to moral violations by politicians more generally. Unfortunately, we have no way to test this speculation. We do find that voters’ responses to these moral violations can be sometimes conditioned by their own moral values, but they are more so by their partisanship when partisan actors are involved. Partisans of both parties express more negative emotions when a politician of the other party violates moral foundations.
While we do not have a direct test of the mechanism by which partisanship conditions the effects of moral violations on emotional responses to politicians, a lengthy literature on partisanship in American politics makes clear that partisans see the political world through a very specific perceptual screen (Campbell et al., 1960). Motivated reasoning (Lodge & Taber, 2013; Redlawsk, 2002) likely becomes engaged when a voter sees a copartisan politician violating a moral foundation, leading to biased processing of the event, and the reduction of negativity about the event. But when the other party commits the violation, partisans are more than willing to express negative emotions about the event. Brain imaging studies reinforce the potential of this mechanism as distinct differences are seen between Democrats and Republicans in their processing of political information (Schreiber et al., 2013). Note that while we started this article with a brief discussion about moral violations by U.S. President Trump and his seeming imperviousness to them, it is worth recalling that it is generally only his Republican supporters who accept his behavior.
This study contributes to the literature in various ways. First of all, it is among a handful studies (Halmburger et al., 2012; Jiang et al., 2011) to study emotional responses to moral violations. A follow‐up study will examine the specific negative emotions to see whether these moral violations evoke specific discrete emotions, in particular so‐called moral emotions (Haidt, 2003). However, our main interest here is to see how partisanship and the importance of moral values for the voter affect emotional responses more generally.
Second, this study is the first to assess the role of voters’ moral values in their response to moral transgressions, as the results show they do matter. However, partisanship, more often than not, overrides the effects of moral values. This is a very interesting finding and suggests that moral foundations are maybe not as innate and foundational as might be supposed (see also, Connors [2019] on political values). The partisanship effect in this study is asymmetrical, showing that voters judge the opposing candidate more harshly for moral transgressions than their own candidate. This is in line with the trend of negative partisanship in American politics (Mason, 2018). Research has shown that while the feelings Democrats and Republicans have about their own party have not changed, their feelings about the opposing party have become much more negative (Abramowitz & Webster, 2016).
As with any research, this study is not without its shortcomings, one being the time frame in which it was conducted, namely following the 2016 presidential elections, which led to the election of a politician with frequent moral transgressions. It is not unthinkable that in a different time period we might find differences with respect to the strength of partisan responses to violations of moral foundations. Thus, we must consider that aspect of our results to be conditional, warranting further study in a different context.
Another limitation is that the vignettes used were specific. That is, the politician referred to in a vignette took a specific action. There is probably heterogeneity in people’s responses to these actions, and by the necessity of the research design, each study participant saw only one action representing one moral foundation. It is possible, for example, that a different vignette representing the same foundation might have a different impact on participants’ emotions. However, we are less interested here in the specific reactions to specific vignettes than we are in the larger story, that moral foundations appear to be readily ignored in the face of partisan actors. Across all of the vignettes we use, partisan voters were far less negative about a same‐party actor violating a foundation than they were an opposite‐party actor. At the same time, although extensively pretested, the authority vignette had the lowest homogeneity coefficient and resulted in the weakest findings. Apparently American voters did not find that specific example to be compelling.
Finally, as described in Footnote 2, new research by Montgomery et al. (2018) suggests that appropriate care must be taken in measuring moderating variables in an experimental context. We were concerned that measuring our moderators—partisanship and moral values—before the experimental treatment would prime participants as to the purpose of our study and thus influence their responses to the treatment. Thus, we measured them at the end of the study. While the very small shared variance we report between the moderators and treatment provides some confidence that our results are not biased, future studies should consider ways to separate these measures, such as the use of multiple waves. If the work by Montgomery et al. (2018) is sustained through additional research, experimentalists in general will need to consider whether the potential costs of such strategies outweigh the risks of biasing results by measuring moderators after an experimental treatment.
While recognizing the potential bias suggested by Montgomery et al. (2018) may be present in our analyses, notwithstanding the very small shared variance between our measures, any such bias would matter more if we were making inferences about the direct effects of specific coefficients in our models. But, instead, we are more interested in the patterns that we see in the data when different moral foundations are engaged across partisan voters. To the extent that these patterns of differential response by partisan voters and those who more or less adopt the moral values of the foundation are biased, we would not expect the bias to eliminate the differences we see across groups. Thus, while the effects of any given treatment may be more or less than we find here, we expect any bias would be unidirectional across the treatments, and thus the patterns we find would remain.
Notwithstanding these limitations, this study makes a significant contribution to the moral foundations literature. For the first time, we examine the intersection of partisanship and moral foundations and find that, as with so many other things in American politics in the early twenty‐first century, responses to moral violations by politicians are subject to partisan preferences. This holds even when voters themselves feel strongly about a given moral foundation. Put simply, when the effects of partisanship and strength of support for moral values are tested against each other in predicting emotional responses to violations of moral foundations, partisanship usually comes out the winner. This provides new insight into the role of moral violations in politics and helps us understand perhaps why some American politicians are able to continue to receive strong support from their own party voters even after violating what are thought to be basic moral values.

Users feel discomfort from exposure to heterogeneity on social media; discomfort can induce them to take action to alleviate their uneasiness; they utilize selective exposure and affective behavior to cope with the discomfort

Feeling displeasure from online social media postings: A study using cognitive dissonance theory. Myeongki Jeong et al. Computers in Human Behavior, Mar 27 2019.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.021

Highlights
•    Users feel discomfort from exposure to heterogeneity on social media.
•    Discomfort can induce users to take action to alleviate their uneasiness.
•    Users utilize selective exposure and affective behavior to cope with the discomfort.
•    The projective technique was used to measure psychological discomfort of the users.

Abstract: Social Network Service (SNS) users are more incidentally exposed to heterogeneous expressions than those engage in off-line discussion networks. The literature has mainly focused on how participation in social media affects users' subjective well-being, while overlooking how the heterogeneous expression on social media affects the users' subjective well-being and consequent behaviors on SNS. Underlining the undesirable consequences of the users’ exposure to opposing views, this study developed a conceptual cognitive framework on the theory of cognitive dissonance, the extreme discomfort of simultaneously holding two conflict thoughts, and showed the existence of mental discomfort of the user in the face of heterogeneous opinions. We also discussed the kind of strategic behaviors the users may take to resolve the uncomfortable psychological state. To test, we first utilized projective technique to measure the uncomfortable psychological states of the respondents, and applied a covariance based structural equation modeling (CB-SEM) methodology with 425 questionnaire responses collected from Prolific Academic (Prolific.ac). We found that the more the users use social media, the more often they are exposed to opposite opinions and the more uncomfortable they feel. In order to resolve these uncomfortable feelings, it appears that these users tend to take selective exposure more than affective behavior. Robustness tests were conducted to check the controlling effects of personality traits and demographic variables on the main variables.

Experiments where participants have to choose between receiving more or less money: A considerable minority chose to receive less; this result holds in different experimental environments

More or Less Money? An Experimental Study on Receiving Money. Sigve Tjøtta. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, MAr 26 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2019.03.007

Highlights
•    I run experiments where participants have to choose between receiving more or less money.
•    A considerable minority chose to receive less money
•    This result holds in different experimental environments
•    This result may compromise interpretation of many experiments.

1 Introduction
A surprising result from one experiment is the point of departure for this paper. The participants were asked to choose between receiving more money or less. The experiment followed standard procedures. I used a randomized payment scheme in which only a subset of the participants were paid. It was a double-blinded design in which neither the participants nor the experimenters could identify the choices made by specific participants. To my surprise, a substantial minority, 28.6% of 91 participants, decided to receive less money. Deciding to receive less money in itself is not a surprise as this is common in many standard experiments such as dictator, ultimatum, and public goods games. Choosing less money in these situations may reflect the decision maker’s strategic behavior or concern toward the other subjects. In the dictator game, choosing less money may reflect concern for the recipient. The strength of the dictator game is that it separates the decision maker’s concern for the recipient from strategic considerations toward the recipient. The more or less money choice goes one step further from the dictator game as it removes explicitconsideration of others participants.If participants in experiments prefer receiving less money than more, the interpretation of economic experiments may be compromised. The result can be explained in at least three ways, and all may affect the interpretations of other experiments. First, subjects’ choice to receive less money, may reflectan experimenter demand effect (Hoffman et al. 1994, Zizzo (2010), and Chlaß and Moffat 2017). Second, subjects may have social preferences toward the experimenter, perceiving they are playing a dictator game with the experimenter. Choosing less money leaves more money for the experimenter. This explanation, however, may also be problematic for the interpretation of experiment results whenever subjects fail to maximize their joint payoff. For
4example, rejecting an offer in an ultimatum game or less trust in a trust game results in the subjects passing on money to the experimenter. Choosing non-cooperation in a public good game increases the payoff for oneself, but it also leaving money to the experimenter. Third, the interpretation of economic experiments is often restricted to a reference group of subjects who play against each other (Fehr and Schmidt 1999:821–822,Bolton and Ockenfels 2000:171).In the more or less money choice the reference groupis reduced to the decision maker only. Therefore, choosing less money may suggest that subjects care about how they perceive themselves. If this self-esteem restricts the subjects’ behavior in more or less money decision, it may also influence their behavior in other experiments. For example, dictators giving to an anonymous recipient is often interpreted, as the dictator preferringequality inthepayoff or following a norm for equityand social-esteem (Ellingsen and Johannesson 2008, Dreber et al.2013, and Hauge 2016).1However, among those that chose to receive less money in my experiment, a clear majority, 88,5 % of the 26 particpants, choose an even split in the corresponding dictator game. Hence, giving in a dictator game can be explained by self-esteem, not social preference or social-esteem, as usually is done.2As I used a randomized payment another selfish explanation is that subjectsdo magical thinking, they believe that claiming less money instead of more increases their probability of winning(Arad 2014). 1For an interpretation of dictator giving as a social preference for inequality see Fehr and Schmidt (1999), Bolton and Ockenfels (2000), Charness and Rabin (2002), Falk and Fischbacher (2006),and Cappelen et al.(2007). This interpretation is an ongoing discussion in the literature (List 2007, Levitt and List 2007, List 2009, Berg and Gigerenzer 2010, Binmore and Shaked 2010, Fehr and Schmidt 2010, Eckel and Gintis 2010, Wilson 2010, Smith and Wilson 2014, and Kimbrough and Ostroknutov 2015).2As the more or less money choice resembles the dictator game, the result that participants prefer getting less money for more are in line with experimental findings that dictator’s giving depend on context (Dana et.al. (2006), List (2007), and Bardsley (2008)).
5If the experimenter demand effect, social preference toward the experimenter, self-esteem, or magical thinkingare that strong in the more or less money experiment, they may also be strong in other experiments. Therefore, the interpretation of manyexperiments may be compromised. Whether these explanations actually carry over to other experiments is an empirical question. Such investigations are beyond the scope of this paper. The aimof this paper is to investigate whether the result that some participants choose to receive less money instead of more holds in other experimental situationsandwhether it translates tothe general population. I createdexperimental situations thatwouldmake accepting more money seem more appropriate for the subjects. Table 1 presents the overall results and features of the decision situations:(1) The experience of participating in experiments (reported in experiments3 and 4). (2) Strengthening the participants’ entitlement to the money (Experiment 5). (3) According to the social heuristic hypothesis, deliberations before the actual choice of more or less money should tilt the decision in the direction of accepting more money (Rand et al. 2014) (experiments6 and 7).The participants were also asked to explain their choice of more or less money in an open-ended question(experiments 6 and 7). Theseopen-ended experiments were performed inthe Norwegian Citizen Panel, a web-based survey of randomized sample of the Norwegian population aged 18 to 76 years. Finally,I elicitednorms in the situations of receiving more or less money using the elicitation procedures developed by Krupka and Weber (2013)(experiments 8 and 9). The overall result is that a considerable minority of the participants in the experiments chose to receive less money instead of more;see Table 1. In Experiment5 -the only one where the participant could choose to receive incrementallyless money-themajority, 64.5% of 200
6participants,decidedto receive less money. In the norm elicitation experiments, aminority stated that choosingto receive all the moneyis inappropriate. I used a randomized payment scheme in all experiments;in each experiment, a percentage of the participants were randomly drawn to receive money. The prices and frequencies of winning are shown in Table 1. For a subject choosing less money instead of more, the expected forgoing of money ranges from, low 0.43 kroner in Experiment 7, to high, 50 kroner in Experiment 5. These numbers correspond to expected hourly payment of 26 and 3000 kroner, respectively.3Clearly, using randomized payment schemes and very low probabilities of being paid in some of the experiments may raise the question whether the result holds for experiments in which all are paid. Manyexperiments, however, use random payment schemes in which either some subjects are paid, or subjects make multiple decisions and only a fraction of them are paid (Azrieli et al., 2018, Charness et al., 2016). It is also some empirical evidence that higher, less likely nominal payment may be more salient than lower, more certain payment (Charness et al., 2016, p. 142). Furthermore, comparing a randomized payment scheme with a standard payment scheme, there seems to be no difference in the pattern of givingin a standard dictator game and behavior in the ultimatum game (Charness et al., 2016 and Clot et al.2018). However, in experiments where the moral dimension may be more salient, as it may be in the more or less money experiment reported here, with a randomized payment schemethe benefit of signaling social esteem or self-esteemis chosen by certainty while the monetary cost of signaling is paid only in a fraction of the times (Charness etal. 2016: 148). This asymmetry between cost and 3The estimated hourly wage is based on that subjects used on average one minute.
7benefit may cause difference in results between experiments with randomized payment schemes and experiments in which all are paid.4Table 1: Overall results for experiments. EXP.N TypeDescription Payment lotteryfrequency Less money1 91ClassroomMore or Less andDictator, within-subjects design500 kroner4/9128.6%2 151Classroom More or Less and Dictator,between-subjects design500 kroner4/15136.0%3, 4181ClassroomExperiencedvs. Inexperiencedsubjects.More or Less and Dictator,between-subjects design500 kroner4/18130.4%5200Lab, 20 sessionsStrengthen Entitlements.More or Less and Dictator,between-subjects design500 kroner1/1064.5%61,019Web-basedaExplaining, ex post their choices in an open-ended question 1800 kroner1/101922.6%71,861Web-basedaDeliberation, Inexperience vs. Experienced, explaining choices ex post and ex ante 1800 kroner1/186129.2%8120Lab, 4 sessionsNorm elicitation500 kroner12/120 19.0%b940ClassroomNorm elicitation 500 kroner10/157 38.0%bNotes: The payment in all the experiments is conditional on winning in a lottery; the column withthePayment Lottery shows the prices in Norwegian kroner and frequency of winning. a)The Norwegian Citizen Panel, a web-based survey of a cross-section of the Norwegian population aged 18 to 76 years old. See Ivarsflaten et al.(2015). b) Percentage that states that it is ―very socially inappropriate‖ or ―socially inappropriate‖ to keep all the money for themselves. The point of departure forthis paper wasratherunusual as it started with asurprising result.In hindsight, perhapsI should not have been surprised becausein someuniversally experienced 4I thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out this mechanism. Note that in Experiment 5, in which the expected cost of signaling is highest (50 kroner), as many as 64.5 % chose to receive less money.
8situationsoutside the lab, people selectless instead of more money. For example, customers return to a shop if they discover that the cashier forgot to charge them for anitem. People routinely take found wallets to the lost and found, and they do likewise in field experiments (Stoop 2014). Similar results are reported in the experimental literature. In a meta-study of 72 studies with more than 32,000 subjects across 43 countries using the lying set-up introduced by Fischbacher and Föllmi-Heusi (2013), the subjects forewent, on average, about three-quarters of the gains from lying (Abeler et al.2016:2). Many participants reported non-maximizing payoff numbers more often than their truthful likelihood (Abeler et al.2016:8). The ―No Die‖ treatment in Fischbacher and Föllmi-Heusi (2013), resembled the more or less choice the most: The participants were asked to pick a number between 1 and 6 with the same incentivized payoff as in the roll die treatment. Among the participants (n=34), 15% chose to receive less money instead of more.Arad (2014) reports that a considerable number of participants in pickingbetween lotteries that differ only in theirprizes, selected lotteries with prizes less than the maximum prize.She explains her results with magical thinking. By choosing the lottery with the lowest price participants believe this would increase their chancesof winning. Magical thinking may be an explanation for choosing less money for more as I usedrandomized payment scheme in all experiments. Among those 454 participants that picked less money in experiments 6 and 7, 10.6% explained their choice as magical thinking. The results presented in this paper may be driven by norms common to Norwegians but less common elsewhere. In general, behavior in experiment has been shown to vary across societies (Henrich et al.2010, Herrmann et al.2008), and itmay also do so in the ―more or less
9money‖ experiment. However behavior in standard experiments in Norwegian samples doesnotdeviate radically from behavior in similar experiments from samples in Western countries. Reigstad et al. (2017) comparedpro-social behavior in different games conducted in Norway and the US and found that pro-social behavior is very similar in the two countries, both across individuals and over time. Cappelen et al. (2015) compared Norwegian students’ behavior in dictator game and found that their behavior was not substantially different from thatreported in the meta-study by Engel (2011). Inexperiments 1-5, I also conducted standard dictator games, and the behavior of the dictator does not deviate radically from the results previously reported in the literature.

Sexual Problems in Long-Term Heterosexual Relationships: The most common and problematic sexual problems endorsed by both sexes were frequency of sex, sexual initiation, & showing interest

Sutherland SE, Rehman US, Fallis EE. A Descriptive Analysis of Sexual Problems in Long-Term Heterosexual Relationships. J Sex Med 2019;XX:XXX–XXX. Mar 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2019.02.015

Abstract
Background: Although much research has described individual sexual dysfunctions, few studies to date have examined the types of problems that couples consider most significant in their sexual relationships.

Aim: To clarify the types of relational sexual problems that are most common and most severe in the sexual lives of individuals in long-term romantic relationships.

Methods: A community sample of 117 mixed-sex couples completed this in-lab study. Members of each couple separately completed a demographics questionnaire and a measure of their relational sexual problems, the Sexual Problems Questionnaire (SPQ). Descriptive analyses (eg, examination of means, frequency counts) were conducted to determine the most common and severe sexual problems reported by participants. t-Tests were performed to examine gender differences in mean severity ratings for each SPQ item. Qualitative data were examined by conducting a frequency count on the SPQ items that participants reported to be most important in their sexual relationships. Results of all frequency counts were divided by the total sample size and are reported as percentages.

Main Outcome Measures: Participants reported on the severity of their sexual problems using the 25-item SPQ.

Results: Quantitative analyses revealed that the most common and problematic sexual problems endorsed by both sexes were frequency of sex, sexual initiation, and showing interest. A frequency count of participants’ qualitative reports also revealed that frequency of sex (women = 36%; men = 39%), sexual initiation (women = 33%; men = 32%), and showing interest (women and men = 25%) were the most important sexual issues for most individuals.

Clinical Implication: The most pressing relational sexual problems for couples in long-term romantic relationships are consistent between sexes and pertain to the domain of sexual desire.

Strength & Limitations: The current study used an expanded measure of sexual problems, which allowed participants to report on a broad range of issues in their sexual relationships. The direction of such relational sexual problems (eg, desiring more or less sexual frequency) was not explored.

Conclusion: The key problems in sexual relationships center on the theme of sexual desire, and men and women consider these issues to be problematic to a similar extent.