Saturday, December 5, 2020

Africa Investment Climate

Africa Investment Climate


Index


1 Background documentation

2 Investment guides

3 Trade Africa-EU

4 Relevant Laws (Commerce, Trade, Business, Land,




1 Background documentation


World Bank


World Bank Annual Report "Doing Business" (the whole world)

https://www.doingbusiness.org/en/reports/global-reports/doing-business-2020



Specific info about Sub-Saharan Africa

https://www.doingbusiness.org/content/dam/doingBusiness/pdf/db2020/DB20-FS-SSA.pdf




United Nations Trade Stats


https://comtrade.un.org/




Official statistical bureaus


Panafrican


The Common Market for Eastern and Southern Africa (COMESA): https://www.comesa.int/




By Country


Benin: https://insae.bj

Burkina Faso: www.douanes.bf/sydoniabf/index.jsf

Burundi: https://www.obr.bi/index.php/en/

Congo: www.cnsee.org

Djibouti: www.ins.ci/n/

Eswatini: www.sra.org.sz

The Gambia: www.motie.gov.gm

Ghana: https://statsghana.gov.gh

Guinea: dgd.gov.gn

Guinea Bissau: www.stat-guinebissau.com

Kenya: https://www.knbs.or.ke

Lesotho: http://www.bos.gov.ls/

Liberia: http://www.moci.gov.lr/

Madagascar: http://www.mefb.gov.mg/

Malawi: http://www.nsomalawi.mw/

Mauritania: http://www.ons.mr/

Morocco: https://www.oc.gov.ma/

Mozambique: http://www.ine.gov.mz/

Namibia: https://nsa.org.na/

Nigeria: https://www.nigerianstat.gov.ng/

Sierra Leone: https://www.statistics.sl/

South Africa: https://www.sars.gov.za/Pages/default.aspx

Tanzania: https://www.nbs.go.tz/index.php/en/

Uganda: https://www.ubos.org/

Zambia: https://www.zamstats.gov.zm/

Zimbabwe: http://www.zimstat.co.zw/











2 Investment guides


2.1 Angola


Investment Guide in Portuguese: https://www.missionangola.ch/telechargements/guia_investimento_angola_isnow.pdf



Information for Angola: Angola Mission in UN and EU

https://www.missionangola.ch



3 Trade Africa-EU


Most African trade is directed to European countries. Some charts, source is the European Commission: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/international-trade-in-goods/data/database



4 Relevant Laws (Commerce, Trade, Business, Land, Labor)


Source: The World Bank.


Information by country. It is only useful to know the climate in the country of interest, but the information is low-quality and incomplete.


In some places at war, like Libya, the former regime didn’t leave in place a legal corpus from which to build. Other places, like Ethiopia, are just now learning how to integrate in a rule-of-law paradigm (at least in form, we don’t know how this will play in the future).


Other places have no applicable laws even in areas in which the modern Eurocentric “colonialist” (so to speak) viewpoint focuses on. An example is a set of environmental laws and regulations, that are still lacking in lots of countries.


Purging, cleaning, filling gaps in these data is a task that will need a review with the embassies and Commerce/Trade Departments in the capital cities of these countries.



Algeria

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws


Angola


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

  • Presidential Decree 64/11, 18th of April

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Benin


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Botswana


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • Insolvency Act [CAP 42:02]

  • Companies Act [CAP 42:01]

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws


Burkina Faso


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws


Burundi


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Cabo Verde


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws

Insolvency Law


Cameroon


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Central African Republic


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

  • **Ordonnance 73/093 fixant les modalités de compression du personnel des services ou organismes publics et para-publics et les entreprises privées de la République Centrafricaine

  • **Ordonnance 73/095 réglementant le recrutement de la main-d'oeuvre en République Centrafricaine

  • **Convention collective nationale du commerce de la République Centrafricaine

  • Code du travail (extraits)

  • Code Du Travail (Extraits)

  • **Convention Collective Nationale Du Commerce De La République Centrafricaine

  • **Ordonnance 73/093 Fixant Les Modalités De Compression Du Personnel Des Services Ou Organismes Publics Et Para-Publics Et Les Entreprises Privées De La République Centrafricaine

  • **Ordonnance 73/095 Réglementant Le Recrutement De La Main-D'Oeuvre En République Centrafricaine

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Chad


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws



Comoros


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Congo, Dem. Rep.


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

  • **Ministerial Decree No. 013/CAB/MIN.URB-HAB/2005 of 6 May 2005 modifies Decree CAB/CE/URB/012/88 of 22 October 1988 that regulates the issuance of the building licence.

Securities Laws

Tax Laws




Congo, Rep.


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Côte d'Ivoire

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Djibouti

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws


Egypt, Arab Rep.


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Equatorial Guinea

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

  • **Ordenamiento General del Trabajo

  • **Ordenamiento General Del Trabajo

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Eritrea


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Eswatini


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws




Ethiopia


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • Ethiopian Commercial Code, 1960 amended by Proclamation No. 6/1991

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Gabon


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Acte Uniforme OHADA du 17 avril 1997 relatif au droit des sociétés commerciales et du groupement d'intérêt économique

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Gambia, The


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • Companies Bill, 2013 

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

  • **Injuries Compensation Act

  • **Labour Act

  • **Injuries Compensation Act

  • **Labour Act

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

  • The Security Interest in Movable Assets Act


Ghana

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Guinea


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws




Guinea-Bissau


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

  • **Lei Geral do Trabalho

  • **Lei Geral Do Trabalho

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Trade and Customs Laws

Insolvency Law

Administrative Laws

Alternative Dispute Resolution Laws

Environmental Laws



Kenya


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws



Lesotho


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Liberia


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws

Insolvency Law




Libya

There is a big hole here.


Commercial and Company Laws



Madagascar


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Malawi


Banking and Credit Laws

  • Credit Reference Bureau (Amendment) Act, 2015

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

  • Commercial Credits Act (Caps 48:06)

  • Hire Purchase Act

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws

Insolvency Law



Mali

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Mauritania


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Mauritius


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws



Morocco

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws




Mozambique

Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Namibia


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • Insolvency Act of 1936

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Niger


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Nigeria


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




Rwanda


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

maternity benefits

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

  • N° 11/2009 of 14/05/2009

  • Nº31/2013 of 24/05/2013




Senegal


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws



Sierra Leone


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




South Africa


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws




South Sudan


Banking and Credit Laws

  • Banking Act 2012

  • Consumer Finance Act 2009

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

  • The Insolvency Act, 2011

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws



Sudan


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws




Tanzania


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws




Tunisia


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws




Uganda


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws

Tax Laws



Zambia


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

Securities Laws




Zimbabwe


Banking and Credit Laws

Bankruptcy and Collateral Laws

Commercial and Company Laws

Labor Laws

Land and Building Laws

  • **Regional, Town and Country Planning Act

Securities Laws

Tax Laws

Trade and Customs Laws




COVID-19 & stay-at-home orders: Users residing in lockdown states create more content after the lockdown, but find a decrease in the novelty level and optimism of the content generated by the latter group

The Unintended Consequences of Stay-at-Home Policies on Work Outcomes: The Impacts of Lockdown Orders on Content Creation. Xunyi Wang, Reza Mousavi, Yili Hong. arXiv, Nov 30 2020. https://arxiv.org/abs/2011.15068

Abstract: The COVID-19 pandemic has posed an unprecedented challenge to individuals around the globe. To mitigate the spread of the virus, many states in the U.S. issued lockdown orders to urge their residents to stay at their homes, avoid get-togethers, and minimize physical interactions. While many offline workers are experiencing significant challenges performing their duties, digital technologies have provided ample tools for individuals to continue working and to maintain their productivity. Although using digital platforms to build resilience in remote work is effective, other aspects of remote work (beyond the continuation of work) should also be considered in gauging true resilience. In this study, we focus on content creators, and investigate how restrictions in individual's physical environment impact their online content creation behavior. Exploiting a natural experimental setting wherein four states issued state-wide lockdown orders on the same day whereas five states never issued a lockdown order, and using a unique dataset collected from a short video-sharing social media platform, we study the impact of lockdown orders on content creators' behaviors in terms of content volume, content novelty, and content optimism. We combined econometric methods (difference-in-differences estimations of a matched sample) with machine learning-based natural language processing to show that on average, compared to the users residing in non-lockdown states, the users residing in lockdown states create more content after the lockdown order enforcement. However, we find a decrease in the novelty level and optimism of the content generated by the latter group. Our findings have important contributions to the digital resilience literature and shed light on managers' decision-making process related to the adjustment of employees' work mode in the long run.



Social Hedonic Editing: People Prefer to Experience Events at the Same Time as Others; excepton are very emotionally impactful events

Social Hedonic Editing: People Prefer to Experience Events at the Same Time as Others. Franklin Shaddy, Yanping Tu, Ayelet Fishbach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, December 4, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620976421

Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1334851536409735169

Abstract: Previous research testing the hedonic editing hypothesis examined preferences for the timing of events that happen to the self—asking, for example, whether people prefer to experience two positive or two negative events on the same or different day(s). Here, we examine preferences for the timing of events that happen to the self and to others—social hedonic editing. Across five studies (N = 2,522), we find people prefer to experience a positive or negative event on the same day that (vs. a different day than) another person experiences a similar positive or negative event. Studies 1 and 2 document this “preference for integration” in interpersonal (i.e., for the self and others) but not intrapersonal (i.e., for the self) contexts, Studies 3 and 4 suggest people prefer integration because it increases interpersonal connection, and Study 5 highlights a boundary condition. People do not prefer integration for very emotionally impactful events.

Keywords: hedonic editing, social decision making, interpersonal connection, event timing


Rolf Degen summarizing... The more people liked something, the more patient they were and the more prepared to accept longer waiting times

Love is patient: People are more willing to wait for things they like. Annabelle Roberts, Franklin Shaddy, and Ayelet Fishbach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General (in press), Dec 2020. https://franklinshaddy.com/assets/liking_patience.pdf

Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1334848015807471618

Abstract: How does liking of a target affect patience? One possibility is that the more people like a target the less patient they are for it, because it is more difficult to resist the attractive smaller-sooner option in order to wait for the larger-later option. However, across six studies (N = 2,774), we found evidence for the opposite effect. Specifically, an increase in liking was correlated with an increase in patience (Study 1), and when people made decisions about a target they liked more, they were more willing to wait for a better quality version of it (Studies 2 and 3) and a larger amount of it (Study 4). This is because when people like a target more, they perceive a greater difference in subjective value between its smaller-sooner and larger-later versions. Thus, the perceived difference in subjective value mediated the effect of liking on patience (Study 5). Further, consistent with this proposed mechanism, we found that liking increased both willingness to wait for a better quality version of a target and willingness to pay to receive the target sooner (Study 6). These findings suggest that patience, in part, results from believing the larger-later reward is worth waiting for. They also offer practical recommendations for people struggling with impatience: Individuals may benefit from reminding themselves why it is they like what they are waiting for.

Keywords: patience, liking, intertemporal choice, subjective value, self-control


Same-sex sexual behaviour can be maintained by selection for indiscriminate sexual behaviour because indiscriminate mating is the optimal strategy under a wide range of conditions

Same-sex sexual behaviour and selection for indiscriminate mating. Brian A. Lerch & Maria R. Servedio. Nature Ecology & Evolution, Nov 9 2020. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41559-020-01331-w

Abstract: The widespread presence of same-sex sexual behaviour (SSB) has long been thought to pose an evolutionary conundrum, as participants in SSB suffer the cost of failing to reproduce after expending the time and energy to find a mate. The potential for SSB to occur as part of an optimal strategy has received less attention, although indiscriminate sexual behaviour may be the ancestral mode of sexual reproduction. Here, we build a simple model of sexual reproduction and create a theoretical framework for the evolution of indiscriminate sexual behaviour. We provide strong support for the hypothesis that SSB can be maintained by selection for indiscriminate sexual behaviour, by showing that indiscriminate mating is the optimal strategy under a wide range of conditions. Further, our model suggests that the conditions that most strongly favour indiscriminate mating were probably present at the origin of sexual behaviour. These findings have implications not only for the evolutionary origins of SSB, but also for the evolution of discriminate sexual behaviour across the animal kingdom.

Empirical observations of SSB (that is, any attempted sexual activity between two or more members of the same sex) in ani-mals are widespread1–4, with evidence of SSB in mammals5–9, birds10–14, arthropods15–19, molluscs20–22, echinoderms23–25 and other animals26–30. Since SSB is traditionally thought to be deleterious, as same-sex matings require energy expenditure but cannot produce offspring, there has been much interest in understanding its origin and maintenance1–5. Despite this, there exists no strong theoretical foundation for understanding SSB (but see refs. 31,32), resulting in a wide range of untested verbal arguments in the literature1–5.Recently, Monk et al.4 challenged the long-standing perspective of SSB as a derived trait, arguing that rather than trying to under-stand its presence, a more salient question would be to understand its absence. They hypothesize that indiscriminate sexual behav-iour (that is, mating without determining the sex of one’s part-ner) is the ancestral condition, realizing that discriminate sexual behaviour (that is, directing sexual behaviour at members of the opposite sex) must evolve through mechanisms controlling sexual signalling and mate choosiness. Of course, the existence of indis-criminate mating as the ancestral condition does not explain its current prevalence33. While in some cases (for example, broadcast spawning and wind pollination) indiscriminate mating predomi-nates as a result of little potential benefit to (or opportunity for) sexual discrimination, it is oftentimes unclear why indiscriminate mating persists.Building on the perspective of Monk et al.4, we argue that selec-tion may actually favour indiscriminate sexual behaviour (or pre-vent the evolution of sexual discrimination) under a wide range of conditions observed in nature. We create a theoretical framework for understanding the conditions that favour indiscriminate sexual behaviour and provide a test of whether SSB is likely to result from selection for indiscriminate sexual behaviour. We start with a sim-ple optimization model of sexual reproduction, and then support this approach with a population genetic model that explicitly tracks evolutionary dynamics. We find that indiscriminate mating is the optimal strategy for many parameter combinations and produce testable predictions about the conditions that favour SSB resulting from indiscriminate mating.

Friday, December 4, 2020

Patients with smell loss with subjective flavor perception might be experiencing unconscious memory recall from previously experienced cross‐modal sensory interactions

Retronasal olfactory function in patients with smell loss but subjectively normal flavor perception. David Tianxiang Liu et al. The Laryngoscope, 130:1629–1633, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/lary.28258

Abstract

Objectives: The human sense of smell constitutes the main part of flavor perception. Typically, patients with loss of olfactory function complain of diminished perception during eating and drinking. However, some patients with smell loss still report normal enjoyment of foods. The aim of the present study was to compare orthonasal and retronasal olfactory function in patients with non‐sinonasal smell loss and subjectively normal flavor perception.

Methods: Nineteen patients (mean age [range] 52.0 [8–83 years]) with self‐reported olfactory impairment but subjective normal flavor perception were included. Olfactory performance was assessed using the Sniffin’ Sticks (TDI) for orthonasal and the Candy Smell Test (CST) for retronasal function. Visual analogue scales were used for self‐assessment of odor (SOP), taste (STP), and flavor perception (SFP), ranging from 0 (no perception) to 10 (excellent perception).

Results: Mean (SD) SFP was 8.0 (1.8). Mean (SD) orthonasal TDI‐score of all patients was 14.4 (5.3, range 6–25.3) with 11 patients classified as anosmic and eight as hyposmic. Mean/SD retronasal CST‐score was 8.8 (2.7, range 3–13) within the range of anosmia/hyposmia. No correlation was found between SFP and the CST (P = .62).

Conclusion: The present results showed that despite claiming normal flavor perception, our patients were ortho‐ and retronasally dysosmic using standard tests for olfactory function. Although other explanations could be possible, we suggest that this subjective flavor perception might be due to unconscious memory recall from previously experienced cross‐modal sensory interactions.


DISCUSSION

An estimated 25% of all people over 50 years of age experience olfactory impairment.562122 A survey from Vennemann et al. on the prevalence of olfactory dysfunctions in the general population showed impairments in almost 18% of the general population with 3.6% classified as functionally anosmic.6 The olfactory system plays the leading role in human multisensory flavor perception,2324 therefore it is expected that a loss of olfactory function leads to altered perception of flavors, which is also confirmed in larger series of patients.25 However, some patients report smell loss but simultaneously state normal to excellent flavor perception. Published26 and unpublished data of our group demonstrate a relatively low percentage of patients with severe olfactory dysfunction but normal subjective flavor perception at the same time (between 3.7% for VAS = 10 and 28% for VAS ≥4).

As the major finding of our study, retronasal olfactory performance as measured by an established retronasal smell test did not confirm normal flavor perception in the investigated subjects. All patients yielded scores within the range of hyposmia/anosmia with ortho‐ and retronasal tests, demonstrating striking discrepancies between subjective and measured flavor identification abilities. In contrast, orthonasal smell test results correlated significantly with self‐assessed olfactory abilities. Our findings are in accordance with current scientific publications stating a moderate but significant correlation between self‐assessment of smell perception and measured olfactory acuity in patients with olfactory dysfunction and confirm a trend that self‐assessment of olfactory function becomes more accurate with decreasing performance.71427 However, it has to be kept in mind, that on an individual patient's level, olfactory performance can only be assessed by means of validated smell tests.28

Regarding gustatory function, the question could arise of whether gustatory function in patients with smell loss and subjectively normal flavor perception is increased, compared to patients with smell loss and concordant loss of flavor perception. This was not found to be the case in our patients, as the majority of achieved TST scores projected in the medium to lower percentile range of normogeusia compared with normative data,16 which is also in accordance with a previously published study showing no significant influence of smell loss on gustatory function.29 As previously described, normosmic patients tend to rely on their odor imagery abilities for self‐assessment of olfactory function7 although this ability seems to decrease with the duration of olfactory loss.3031 A tendency of these patients to rely more on gustatory, textural, auditory (during mastication), and visual information of foods could be a reason for the lack of correlation between self‐assessment and test results of retronasal olfactory function.32 Our findings show that relying exclusively on subjective reports on flavor perception in patients with olfactory dysfunction can be misleading and additional testing of retronasal olfactory function can provide more information for the management regarding hazardous events (eg, ingestion of spoiled food).33

Why does the loss of retronasal olfactory function go unnoticed in some patients? Although we cannot give answers to this question based on our results, some thoughts might be relevant for further research. In our patients subjectively normal flavor perception during food intake was not mediated by intact retronasal olfactory function. In another investigation retronasal olfactory event‐related potentials could be recorded from some patients with unimpaired flavor perception which were ortho‐ and retronasally tested to be dysosmic by means of psychophysical tests.14 However, this might not be clinically relevant, since olfactory event‐related potentials can also be present in patients with functional anosmia, for whom residual olfactory function is not useful in everyday life.171834

Part of the contribution of retronasal smell stimuli to overall flavor perception seems to be mediated by memory recall. Therefore unconscious memory recall of “flavor templates” from previously experienced cross‐modal sensory interactions (eg, somatosensory–olfactory interactions) may be an explanation for normal flavor perception in orthonasally anosmic patients with noncongenital causes.303536 All three patients in our study with congenital smell loss yielded scores within the range of anosmia in ortho‐ and retronasal tests presuming “flavor” is an individual concept, consisting of interaction of all other sensory modalities (for example vision, taste, sound, and somatosensory) independently from olfactory perception. Long‐term olfactory recognition memory, which plays a vital role in food preference and food habits, happens unconsciously and incidentally through repeated presentation of individual components together, as is the case with food and beverages.37-39 A further mentionable point is that the development of our multisensory flavor perception probably already starts in the mother's womb39 and continues into adulthood. The frequent presentation and co‐occurrence of olfactory stimuli with other sensory stimuli, eg, of gustatory and olfactory quality, consequently allow qualities of one sensory system to evoke qualities in another.40 Further studies using functional imaging methods, for example, are needed for more clarity regarding different brain activities with variability of self‐assessment of different sensory modalities.

Finally, as shown in a recent publication, olfactory changes are not as strongly perceived as visual changes. While olfactory changes were only detected with an accuracy of 61%, visual changes were detected with an accuracy of over 97%. Only 24% of the participants were able to detect olfactory changes reliably above chance. Notably, these subjects also rated their personal interest in olfaction and its use in daily life as most important.41 Regarding our subgroup of patients with smell loss and no subjective change in flavor perception, it might be speculated that these patients rely more on visual, gustatory, and trigeminal cues during eating and drinking leading to an unawareness of a decreased retronasal odor identification ability.

Neoliberal economics, planetary health, and the COVID-19 pandemic: a Marxist ecofeminist analysis

Neoliberal economics, planetary health, and the COVID-19 pandemic: a Marxist ecofeminist analysis. Simon Mair. The Lancet, December, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30252-7

Abstract: Planetary health sees neoliberal capitalism as a key mediator of socioecological crises, a position that is echoed in much COVID-19 commentary. In this Personal View, I set out an economic theory that emphasises some of the ways in which neoliberal capitalism's conceptualisation of value has mediated responses to COVID-19. Using the intersection of ecological, feminist, and Marxist economics, I develop an analysis of neoliberal capitalism as a specific historical form of the economy. I identify the accumulation of exchange value as a central tendency of neoliberal capitalism and argue that this tendency creates barriers to the production of other forms of value. I then analyse the implications of this tendency in the context of responses to COVID-19. I argue that resources and labour flow to the production of exchange value, at the expense of production of other value forms. Consequently, the global capitalist economy has unprecedented productive capacity but uses little of this capacity to create the conditions that improve and maintain people's health. To be more resilient to coming crises, academics, policy makers, and activists should do theoretical work that enables global economies to recognise multiple forms of value and political work that embeds these theories in societal institutions.


Key messages

  • The economy is the system by which a society takes in resources and uses them to produce and distribute goods and services.
  • Neoliberal capitalism is a particular structuring of the economy that prioritises exchange value above other types of value.
  • Prioritising exchange value has led neoliberal capitalism to develop unprecedented productive capacity.
  • Neoliberal capitalism primarily uses its productive capacity to produce more exchange value. This process undermines other value forms, including health.
  • Effective responses to COVID-19 prioritise health and life and undermine exchange value.
  • To be better prepared for future pandemics and other crises, global society should build economies that can recognise multiple forms of value.

While men's orgasm consistency is linearly associated with relational and sexual satisfaction, for women, with each unit increase in orgasm consistency, the increase in those satisfactions became progressively smaller

Leavitt CE, Leonhardt ND, Busby DM, et al. When Is Enough Enough? Orgasm's Curvilinear Association With Relational and Sexual Satisfaction. J Sex Med Dec 4 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.10.002

Abstract

Background: Curvilinearity has been found for sexual frequency, but research has not examined whether curvilinear associations exist for other aspects of sexual relationships like orgasm consistency.

Aim: We examined whether there is curvilinearity and the nature of that curvilinearity between orgasm consistency and sexual and relational satisfaction for men and women.

Methods: With pooled samples of 1,619 and 1,695 men and women from Amazon's Mechanical Turk, we examined the differences of orgasm consistency values and both sexual satisfaction and relationship satisfaction through analysis of variance. We then tested for curvilinearity between orgasm consistency and sexual and relational satisfaction with regression analyses.

Outcomes: For men we found no evidence of a curvilinear relationship, but for women we found a curvilinear relationship between orgasm consistency values and both sexual satisfaction and relationship satisfaction.

Results: Across tests, the overall picture suggests that there is no curvilinear association for men, but there is for women. For women, with each unit increase in orgasm consistency, the increase in sexual satisfaction and relationship satisfaction became progressively smaller. Past the 61-80% threshold for orgasm consistency, there was little gain in sexual satisfaction and no gain in relational satisfaction.

Clinical Translation: Physicians, therapists, and educators can reorient women's orgasm expectations by explaining that having regular orgasms—not necessarily always—is associated with satisfaction in their relationship and sexual experience.

Strengths & Limitations: Converging large samples and data analytic techniques evinced the curvilinear association between orgasm consistency and both relational and sexual satisfaction for women. However, this study is cross-sectional and correlational, which limits the conclusions we can draw from it.

Conclusion: While men's orgasm consistency is linearly associated with relational and sexual satisfaction, more consistent orgasms seem to be associated with women's sexual and relational satisfaction, to a point.

Key Words: OrgasmSexual SatisfactionRelational Satisfaction


Short-term mating was unrelated or even negatively related to reproductive success; conversely, long-term mating was positively associated with reproductive success

Međedović, Janko M. 2020. “Reproductive Ecology of Short and Long-term Mating: Implications for Sexual Selection and Life History Theory.” PsyArXiv. December 4. doi:10.31234/osf.io/6zcnm

Abstract: Mating patterns are crucial for understanding selection regimes in current populations and highly implicative for sexual selection and life history theory. However, empirical data on the relations between mating and fitness-related outcomes in contemporary humans are lacking. In the present research we examined the sexual selection on mating (with an emphasis on Bateman’s third parameter – the association between mating and reproductive success) and life history dynamics of mating by examining the relations between mating patterns and a comprehensive set of variables which determine human reproductive ecology. We conducted two studies (Study 1: N=398, Mage=31.03; Study 2: N=996, Mage=40.81, the sample was representative for participants’ sex, age, region, and settlement size). The findings from these studies were mutually congruent and complementary. In general, the data suggested that short-term mating was unrelated or even negatively related to reproductive success. Conversely, long-term mating was positively associated with reproductive success and there were indices that the beneficial role of long-term mating is more pronounced in males, which is in accordance with Bateman’s third principle. Observed age of first reproduction fully mediated the link between long-term mating and number of children but only in male participants. There were no clear indications of the position of the mating patterns in human life history trajectories; however, the obtained data suggested that long-term mating has some characteristics of fast life history dynamics. Findings are implicative for sexual selection and life history theory in humans.


LGBs show higher in levels of political interest, turnout & other forms of political participation in Western Europe over & above what can be determined by socio-economic determinants of political participation

Political engagement and turnout among same-sex couples in Western Europe. Stuart J. Turnbull-Dugarte, Joshua Townsley. Research & Politics, December 3, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168020976952

Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1334744960025980929

Abstract: This paper presents and addresses a simple, yet overlooked, research question: is there a sexuality gap in political engagement and participation between sexual minority individuals and the heterosexual majority in Western Europe? To answer this question, we employ a recently applied method of identifying lesbian, gay and bisexual (LGB) individuals using data on the gender composition of cohabiting partner households from the European Social Survey. Relying on a total sample of more than 110,000 individuals across 12 different countries with an identified sample of 1542 LGB individuals, we test the divergence in political interest and political participation, both electoral and non-electoral, between LGB and non-LGB individuals. The results of our empirical analyses conform with our expectations. Theorising that LGBs, as a marginalised social stratum, are incentivised to participate and ‘vote like their rights depended on it’, we find empirical evidence of a significant and positive ‘sexuality gap’ in levels of political interest, turnout and other forms of political participation in Western Europe over and above what can be determined by socio-economic determinants of political participation.

Keywords Political interest, elections, participation, engagement, sexuality, LGBT+


A core feature of political behaviour that has hitherto not been considered within the electorates of Europe is whether or not one’s sexuality is likely to affect the propensity of an individual to be engaged in politics to participate in the democratic process. Our analyses show that, on average and across Western Europe, LGB citizens (those in a same-sex relationship) are significantly more likely to be active participants in democratic politics than comparable heterosexuals (those in opposite sex relationships). There is, therefore, an independent ‘sexuality gap’ in political behaviour that cannot be explained by traditional socio-economic determinants of participation.

We acknowledge the potential limitations of the analysis. Firstly, given that the measurement strategy relies on individuals being in a relationship, the extent to which the effects can be generalised across those who are and are not currently in a relationship is unclear (Kühne et al., 2019). Secondly, we are not able to distinguish between those who are bisexual and those who are gay or lesbian. Although Schnabel (2018) argues that this within-group asymmetry is likely minimal, we acknowledge the potential heterogeneity between these distinctive subgroups (Swank, 2018Worthen, 2020). Ultimately, given the strategy applied, we are unable to differentiate between LGs and Bs. Notwithstanding these limitations, however, establishing that sexuality increases both political interest and the propensity of individuals to participate in national elections creates vast avenues for additional research. Given the infancy of scholarship concerning the individual-level behaviour of this particular minority group, most notably in Europe, assessing whether and how the assumptions regarding minority group behaviour travel across groups that are structured by sexuality provides for an interesting subfield within the discipline yet to be fully explored.

Bystanders help immediately when they are alone but help later & are less likely to help when part of a larger group; those in need of help are helped earlier & are more likely to be helped in larger groups because it is easier to find an altruistic guy

The volunteer’s dilemma explains the bystander effect. Pol Campos-Mercade. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, December 3 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.11.012

Abstract: The bystander effect is the phenomenon that people are less likely to help others when they are in a group than when they are alone. The theoretical literature typically explains the bystander effect with the volunteer’s dilemma: if providing help is equivalent to creating a public good, then bystanders could be less likely to help in groups because they free ride on the other bystanders. This paper uses a dynamic game to experimentally test such strategic interactions as an explanation for the bystander effect. In line with the predictions of the volunteer’s dilemma, I find that bystanders help immediately when they are alone but help later and are less likely to help if they are part of a larger group. In contrast to the model’s predictions, subjects in need of help are helped earlier and are more likely to be helped in larger groups. This finding can be accounted for in an extended model that includes both altruistic and selfish bystanders. The paper concludes that the volunteer’s dilemma is a sensible way to model situations in which someone is in need of help, but it highlights the need to take heterogeneous social preferences into account.

Keywords: Volunteer’s dilemmaBystander effectHelping behaviorGroup sizeAltruism


Empathy and Schadenfreude in Human–robot Teams

de Jong, Dorina, Ruud Hortensius, Te-Yi Hsieh, and Emily S. Cross. 2020. “Empathy and Schadenfreude in Human–robot Teams.” PsyArXiv. December 3. doi:10.31234/osf.io/2wyxp

Abstract: Intergroup dynamics shape the ways in which we interact with other people. We feel more empathy towards ingroup members compared to outgroup members, and can even feel pleasure when an outgroup member experiences misfortune, known as schadenfreude. Here, we test the extent to which these intergroup biases emerge during interactions with robots. We measured trial-by-trial fluctuations in emotional reactivity to the outcome of a competitive reaction time game to assess both empathy and schadenfreude in arbitrary human-human and human-robot teams. Across four experiments (total n = 361), we observed a consistent empathy and schadenfreude bias driven by team membership. People felt more empathy towards ingroup members than outgroup members and more schadenfreude towards outgroup members. The existence of an intergroup bias did not depend on the nature of the agent: the same effects were observed for human-human and human–robot teams. People reported similar levels of empathy and schadenfreude towards a human and robot player. The human likeness of the robot did not consistently influence this intergroup bias, however, similar empathy and schadenfreude biases were observed for both humanoid and mechanical robots. For all teams, this bias was influenced by the level of team identification; individuals who identified more with their team showed stronger intergroup empathy and schadenfreude bias. Together, we show that similar intergroup dynamics that shape our interactions with people can also shape interactions with robots. Our results highlight the importance of taking intergroup biases into account when examining social dynamics of human-robot interactions.


Thursday, December 3, 2020

Men perceived affirmative action more negatively than women, but women were more put off by being rejected rather than selected based on gender

Carlsson, Rickard, and Samantha Sinclair. 2020. “Preprint_selected or Rejected: Men and Women’s Reactions to Affirmative Action Procedures in Hiring.” PsyArXiv. December 3. doi:10.31234/osf.io/32mun

Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1334538000680841216

Abstract: Previous research suggests that affirmative action policies tend to be perceived more negatively by men than by women, and by non-beneficiaries relative to beneficiaries. However, studies focusing on men as beneficiaries are lacking. The present paper reports the results of two pre-registered experiments conducted in Sweden. Study 1 investigated gender differences in reactions to being selected for a position based on either a strong or weak type of affirmative action policy. The results revealed that men (relative to women) displayed more negative attitudes, but not stronger resentment, and that a procedure using explicit quotas was perceived more negatively than a softer type of preferential treatment. In Study 2, we experimentally manipulated whether participants imagined being selected or rejected due to the same preferential treatment policy. Again, men displayed more negative attitudes, but not stronger resentment. The results further showed that attitudes were negative regardless of whether one was selected or rejected. However, those who were rejected felt stronger resentment than those who were selected, and this effect was especially pronounced for women. Implications for research, organizations, and policy-makers are discussed.


High cognition helps more a team when there is high category heterogeneity, high external interdependence, low authority differentiation, temporal dispersion, and geographic dispersion

Conditioning team cognition: A meta-analysis. Ashley A. Niler et al. Organizational Psychology Review, December 3, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386620972112

Abstract: Abundant research supports a cognitive foundation to teamwork. Team cognition describes the mental states that enable team members to anticipate and to coordinate. Having been examined in hundreds of studies conducted in board rooms, cockpits, nuclear power plants, and locker rooms, to name a few, we turn to the question of moderators: Under which conditions is team cognition more and less strongly related to team performance? Random effects meta-analytic moderator analysis of 107 independent studies (N = 7,778) reveals meaningful variation in effect sizes conditioned on team composition and boundary factors. The overall effect of team cognition on performance is ρ = .35, though examining this effect by these moderators finds the effect can meaningfully vary between ρ = .22 and ρ = .42. This meta-analysis advances team effectiveness theory by moving past the question of “what is important?” to explore the question of “when and why is it important?” Results indicate team cognition is most strongly related to performance for teams with social category heterogeneity (ρ = .42), high external interdependence (ρ = .41), as well as low authority differentiation (ρ = .35), temporal dispersion (ρ = .36), and geographic dispersion (ρ = .35). Functional homogeneity and temporal stability (compositional factors) were not meaningful moderators of this relationship. The key takeaway of these findings is that team cognition matters most for team performance when—either by virtue of composition, leadership, structure, or technology—there are few substitute enabling conditions to otherwise promote performance.

Keywords: team cognition, team composition attributes, team boundary attributes, team performance, meta-analysis


Who Cares If You Vote? We find that citizens are more likely to perceive normative pressure to vote from fellow partisans

Who Cares If You Vote? Partisan Pressure and Social Norms of Voting. Edward Fieldhouse, David Cutts & Jack Bailey. Political Behavior, Dec 3 2020. https://rd.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11109-020-09661-y

Abstract: Social norms are important in explaining why people vote, but where do those norms come from and is social pressure motivated by partisanship? In this article, we use political discussion network data to examine the role of party identification in shaping the relationship between injunctive norms, civic duty and voter turnout. More specifically, we examine the extent to which both the application of injunctive norms and their impact on turnout is affected by shared partisan identification. We find that citizens are more likely to perceive normative pressure to vote from fellow partisans, a phenomenon we refer to as “partisan pressure”. However we do not find consistent evidence for the hypothesis that turnout is more closely related to the approval or disapproval of discussants who share a partisanship. By separating the role of social pressure from that of normative beliefs we also demonstrate that injunctive norms affect voter turnout both directly and indirectly by increasing civic duty.


Discussion

Existing research suggests that it is difficult to explain why people vote without invoking the concept of social norms. However, due in part to the limited availability of relational data, political scientists have tended to focus on personal normative beliefs, especially civic duty, ignoring the role of normative influence, either directly on voting or indirectly via civic duty. This has made it difficult to understand the process of normative influence, including who is most likely to apply normative pressure, and whether or not some political discussion partners are more influential than others. In this article we have presented a number of new and important findings concerning the relationships between injunctive norms, civic duty and turnout and how these relationships are shaped by party identification. For the first time we have demonstrated the role of shared partisan identification in determining the extent of social pressure to vote. We found strong evidence for partisan pressure—that is that norms are selectively enforced (Hypothesis 1). In short, people are more likely to perceive that their fellow partisans care whether they vote than discussants who do not share a party affiliation, especially identifiers of opposing parties. Second, we demonstrated that this social pressure is associated with higher levels of civic duty (Hypothesis 2), which in turn is correlated with voter turnout. Although the size of the effect of social pressure on duty (the coefficient) was not found to vary by dyadic partisanship (Hypothesis 3), because the amount of social pressure is higher in shared partisan dyads, the level of duty also tends to be higher. Moreover, despite the importance of social pressure in shaping civic duty, approval of voting was found to be strongly associated with turnout even after allowing for civic duty (Hypothesis 4). However, we do not find consistent evidence that social pressure from fellow partisans was more strongly related to turnout than that from opposing partisans (Hypothesis 5). The failure to consistently identify the differential effect may reflect either the unique electoral context of our primary dataset (2014), peculiarities of the supplementary samples (2015 and 2017), or both.

It is important to remember that these findings relate to interpersonal influence through dyadic injunctive norms and partisanship, not to the character of whole networks (Huckfeldt et al. 2004; Nir 2011). While we find that shared partisanship stimulates social pressure to vote, it is entirely plausible that disagreement could simultaneously stimulate turnout through increased partisan discussion (Foos and de Rooij 2016). The latter, as Foos and de Rooij point out, may reflect friendly competition (‘I will vote to offset your vote’) whilst the former relies on a desire for co-operation (‘you should vote because our party needs your support’).

Before accepting these conclusions, we should consider possible problems of endogeneity that threaten causal inference. First, regarding the effect of shared partisanship on approval, it might be argued that there is reciprocal causality. This would require that respondents select discussants that care about their turnout behaviour and, in turn, this would make them more likely to adopt a shared a party preference. In other words, the showing of approval of turnout by a discussant would have to influence the party identification of the respondent. This seems rather unlikely. Certain types of discussant may well be both more likely to approve and likely to influence the party preference of respondent, but this must be attributable to some characteristics of the dyad (e.g. the closeness of the relationship) and is therefore a problem of omitted variable bias rather than reverse causality. To mitigate this, we have included a substantial range of individual and dyadic controls. It is hard to think of many other characteristics not already included in the approval model that might be related to both shared partisanship and approval of voting. However, we acknowledge there may be omitted variables that are related to both sharing partisanship and applying normative pressure which means that causal inference needs to be made with caution. Regarding the relationship between approval and turnout, similar arguments apply.

Another possible threat to inference is that civic duty and turnout of the respondent may influence the likelihood of perceiving approval amongst discussants. However, there is no theoretical or logical reason that any misperception should be restricted to those sharing the same partisanship. The fact that the inclusion of a large number of other individual and dyadic controls makes little difference to the effect of dyadic partisanship reassures us that dyadic partisanship most likely does influence perceived approval of voting. Whilst endogeneity can rarely be ruled out in observational cross-sectional analysis, it is important to note that the existence of endogeneity per se does not necessarily render analysis obsolete. By its very nature, influence via social norms and social networks is an endogenous process. People influence each other, and this is precisely our object of interest. If we are to understand these routes of influence better, then it is necessary to recognise and acknowledge this and employ relational data as we have here. Indeed, whilst experimental designs have proved very successful at identifying causal effects (e.g. Sinclair 2012) they have more limited potential for uncovering the underlying patterns of normative influence. Network designs may be inferior for making causal inference, but arguably better suited for describing the conditions under which normative pressure is applied—an important and sometimes undervalued endeavour (Blais and Daoust 2020; Hersh and Ghitza 2018). The network data used here has enabled us to come to an important conclusion: citizens are more likely to perceive normative pressure from fellow partisans.

In sum, we believe the findings of this research are important in furthering our understanding of how social norms of voting operate, and the crucial role of partisanship. This matters because models of turnout are heavily reliant on the concepts of norms and, very often, this is reduced to personal normative beliefs, especially civic duty, which is frequently treated as an exogenous characteristic of autonomous and isolated citizens. We suggest that this is unsatisfactory: norms are social constructs and, if normative beliefs of voting are critical to understanding turnout, then we must at least attempt to understand more about where they come from and when they are most effective. Here we have taken a small step in that direction, making a clear link between party identity and normative influence on both civic duty and voter turnout.

Contrary to existing research, both liberals and conservatives are more fearful of different circumstances

Dispositional Fear and Political Attitudes. Peter K. Hatemi & Rose McDermott. Human Nature, Dec 3 2020. https://rd.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12110-020-09378-1

Abstract: Previous work proposes that dispositional fear exists predominantly among political conservatives, generating the appearance that fears align strictly along party lines. This view obscures evolutionary dynamics because fear evolved to protect against myriad threats, not merely those in the political realm. We suggest prior work in this area has been biased by selection on the dependent variable, resulting from an examination of exclusively politically oriented fears that privilege conservative values. Because the adaptation regulating fear should be based upon both universal and ancestral-specific selection pressures combined with developmental and individual differences, the elicitation of it should prove variable across the ideological continuum dependent upon specific combinations of fear and value domains. In a sample of ~ 1,600 Australians assessed with a subset of the Fear Survey Schedule II, we find fears not infused with political content are differentially influential across the political spectrum. Specifically, those who are more fearful of sharp objects, graveyards, and urinating in public are more socially conservative and less supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of death are more supportive of gay rights. Those who are more fearful of suffocating and swimming alone are more concerned about emissions controls and immigration, while those who are more fearful of thunderstorms are also more anti-immigration. Contrary to existing research, both liberals and conservatives are more fearful of different circumstances, and the role of dispositional fears are attitude-specific.


There is less of a disproportionate conservative antipathy toward protests than of a disproportionate liberal enthusiasm

Clarifying the Ideological Asymmetry in Public Attitudes Toward Political Protest. David Barker, Kimberly Nalder, Jessica Newham. American Politics Research, December 2, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X20975329

Rolf Degen's take: https://twitter.com/DegenRolf/status/1334383061102977025

Abstract: Political protests cannot succeed without public support. Extant studies point to weaker average support among ideological conservatives, but researchers have yet to consider the extent to which such apparent ideological asymmetry is (a) an artifact of the particular protest cases that researchers have tended to investigate, and/or (b) conditioned by the precise meaning of “ideological conservatism.” In this investigation, we address these gaps. Specifically, we analyze public perceptions of protest legitimacy after exposing survey respondents to one of a series of experimental treatments that randomize the specific ideological and issue contents of the particular protests under consideration. In iterative models, we observe how political ideology, social dominance orientation and authoritarianism condition the effects associated with these experimental treatments. The data suggest that that the notorious ideological asymmetry that is often associated with support for protests is authentic, but it is also conditioned in important ways by these other factors.

Keywords: political protest, attitudes, ideology, authoritarianism, social dominance orientation


Wednesday, December 2, 2020

Conservative opposition to refugee resettlement can be weakened if conservatives are given reasons to believe those refugees will support the Republican Party; liberal support for refugees drops when they receive that information

Cui Bono? Partisanship and Attitudes Toward Refugees. Richard Hanania. Social Science Quarterly, November 30 2020. https://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.12891

Abstract

Objective: This paper tests the hypothesis that the expected partisan affiliation of refugee populations partially explains why white conservatives and white liberals have different attitudes toward refugee resettlement in the United States.

Method: This was tested with a preregistered survey experiment that examined how attitudes toward refugee resettlement changed depending on the racial and political characteristics of a theoretical refugee population.

Results: Conservative opposition to refugee resettlement can be weakened if conservatives are given reasons to believe those refugees will support the Republican Party. At the same time, liberal support for refugees drops when they receive the same information.

Conclusion: Although white conservatives and white liberals exhibit different levels of racial prejudice, and this has consequences for their immigration and refugee policy preferences, their beliefs about how newcomers influence domestic partisan politics are also consequential.


Cultural anthropology’s love-hate relationship with evolution: what will the future bring?

van Schaik, Carel P (2020). Cultural anthropology’s love-hate relationship with evolution: what will the future bring? Zeitschrift für Ethnologie, 144:77-92. https://www.zora.uzh.ch/id/eprint/192455/

Abstract: Cultural anthropology and evolutionary biology arose around the same time, and both adopted the same evolutionist framework. Their paths soon diverged, however, largely because anthropology rejected the notion of evolutionary progress—and thus the notion of the existence of primitive versus advanced races—before evolutionary biology did. Most anthropologists subsequently rejected all evolutionary interpretations of ethnographic patterns and thus all biological influences on human behavior. Most evolutionary biologists until recently ignored the massive role of culture in guiding human behavior. Promising recent work suggests that important new insights emerge when evolutionary and cultural influences on behavior and society are integrated. The success of these new approaches indicates that the presence of a similar mental substrate everywhere produces a non-trivial level of predictability and thus convergence in cultural evolution. Future work along these lines should therefore yield novel insights in how humans respond to changing subsistence or institutional arrangements.