Sunday, February 27, 2022

Depression and suicidality as evolved credible signals of need in social conflicts

Depression and suicidality as evolved credible signals of need in social conflicts. Michael R. Gaffney et al. Evolution and Human Behavior, February 25 2022. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.02.004

Abstract: Mental health professionals generally view major depression and suicidality as pathological responses to stress that elicit aversive responses from others. An alternative hypothesis grounded in evolutionary theory contends that depression and suicidality are honest signals of need in response to adversity that can increase support from reluctant others when there are conflicts of interest. To test this hypothesis, we examined responses to emotional signals in a preregistered experimental vignette study involving claims of substantial need in the presence of conflicts of interest and private information about the signaler's true level of need. In a sample of 1240 participants recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk, costlier signals like depression and suicidality increased perceptions of need, reduced perceptions of manipulativeness, and increased likelihood of support compared to simple verbal requests and crying without further symptoms. The effect of signaling on likelihood of support was largely mediated by the effect of signaling on participants' belief that the signaler was genuinely in need. Our results support the hypothesis that depression and suicidality, apparent human universals, are credible signals of need that elicit more support than verbal requests, sad expressions, and crying when there are conflicts of interest.

Keywords: DepressionSuicideEvolutionary medicineCostly signalingMental health

4. Discussion

As predicted, in vignettes involving conflicts of interest and private information about the need for help, costly signals of need increased participants' belief in the victim's claims and their likelihood of helping her, with the increase in belief and the likelihood of helping increasing monotonically with signal cost. As predicted, the increase in likelihood of helping was largely mediated by the increase in belief in the victim's claims. In an exploratory analysis, costlier signals also decreased perceptions that the victim was manipulative. These results provide evidence that, contrary to the influential “interpersonal” view that depressive behaviors are socially dysfunctional (reviewed in Hames et al., 2013), they in fact outperform verbal requests, sad expressions, and crying in providing benefits to victims when there are conflicts of interest.

Signal effects were largest in the “brother-in-law” and “romantic partner” vignettes, both of which involved claims of assault against participants' imagined daughters, and smaller in the “basketball coach” and the “thwarted marriage” vignettes. The smaller effect in the “basketball coach” vignette might have been because in the role of athletic director, participants did not value their relationship with the star player as much as we anticipated (e.g., due to lack of relatedness), or how participants weighted the costs of suspending the coach vs. punishing a potentially innocent person (for discussion of suicidal signaling to kin vs. nonkin, see Syme & Hagen, 2018). The US vignettes also had different degrees of evidence against the victim beyond just denial by the accused, ranging from strong evidence in the basketball coach vignette (a negative police report) to moderate evidence in the romantic partner vignette (no physical injuries) to weak evidence in the brother-in-law vignette (nothing beyond denial by the brother-in-law), raising the possibility that credible signals are more effective when negative evidence is lacking (Dylan Tweed, personal communication).

The small signal effect in the “thwarted marriage” vignette, which involved the Indian sample, could indicate that our results do not generalize across cultures, undermining our adaptationist hypothesis. It could also reflect our poor understanding of contemporary Indian culture regarding dowry (the effect was larger in older participants). Baseline belief in the older daughter, and likelihood of helping her, was relatively high at baseline (58%) compared to victims in the other vignettes. Private information and conflict therefore probably played a smaller role and thus costly signals were less necessary. We observed a similar pattern in our pilot study, in which baseline belief in the victim's need was high, and costly signals had smaller effects than they did in the current study. Additionally, supporting the older daughter came at the cost of one's younger daughter, which may also help explain the relatively small signal effects. A final consideration is that data from the Indian sample appeared to be of lower quality, limiting our confidence in any of these interpretations (see the Limitations section for more information).

In the “brother-in-law” and “romantic partner” scenarios, Crying had little effect on the magnitude of pro-victim responses relative to Verbal request, suggesting it was not costly enough to serve as a reliable signal in times of substantial conflicts of interests. In contrast, both Depression conditions increased support, albeit to similar degrees. One potential reason for the similar effects of the Depression conditions is the increase in costs from Mild depression to Depression was small (e.g., grades dropping from As to Bs in Mild depression vs. Cs in Depression). Such small changes may be less impactful in vignettes than in real-life, where the effects of signaling may increase in severity as they persist over time.

The effect of Suicide attempt on T2 Belief was similar to the Depression conditions across vignettes, but it resulted in greater T2 Action. One interpretation is that although some participants did not believe the victim's story, her signal nevertheless convinced them that she needed help. For example, maybe the brother-in-law did not assault her, but the presence of his family in her home was causing genuine distress. Support for this interpretation comes from our mediation analyses, which showed that the likelihood of help was largely, but not entirely, mediated by signal's effect on belief in need.

There were minor associations of age and sex with T2 Belief and T2 Action in the US participants, with both being higher among females and younger individuals. The US vignettes all involved assaults against young women, which might have been more salient to female and younger participants. In the Indian sample, T2 Belief and T2 Action were somewhat lower among those with more education and among females, respectively. Costlier signals, suicidality in particular, had a larger effect among older individuals, perhaps because older individuals were more likely to have children of marriageable age, like the victim in the vignette.

Contrary to our adaptationist hypothesis, and supporting the mainstream view that depression is a psychopathology, participants' perceptions that the victim was mentally ill increased with signal cost. However, there have been extensive media campaigns to convince the public that depression is a mental illness with the laudable goal of reducing stigma (Corrigan, 2012Rüsch, Angermeyer, & Corrigan, 2005). Even so, in the Depression conditions across vignettes, no more than 25% of participants thought the victim was mentally ill, and in the Suicide attempt condition the proportion of participants perceiving mental illness exceeded 50% only in the basketball coach vignette. Although perceived mental illness was associated with somewhat lower T2 Belief and T2 Action, this effect was mainly evident in the Verbal request and Crying conditions.

Finally, after the T3 evidence that the victim was telling the truth, likelihood of helping by the US participants increased to near ceiling, an effect that helped validate our vignettes. Among Indian participants, in contrast, participants only slightly increased their likelihood of helping from their T2 level. One interpretation of the latter is that Indian participants tended to believe the older daughter anyway, so their decision to help was not changed by additional information.

4.1. Limitations

This study has less ecological validity than real-world observations of depressed individuals interacting with their social partners, which might have biased results in a pro-signaler direction if the lack of real costs of helping made support feel less costly or if there was a social desirability bias toward helping (Grimm, 2010). It may have also biased participants against helping if they could not fully imagine the characters in the story as kin or interdependent partners, and the survey's short duration may have weakened the strength of the costlier signals as bargaining tools.

Our design did not include vignettes with male signalers. For this reason, we have no data on the possibility of sex differences in the effectiveness of the signaling strategies examined. Although not predicted theoretically, such differences are possible if the costs of signaling vary between the sexes due to differential access to alternative bargaining strategies (Hagen & Rosenström, 2016) or if one sex tends to suffer greater negative reputational effects when displaying the emotions and behaviors in the vignettes. It is also possible the costliness of the situations presented in the vignettes differ by sex. This study therefore most clearly demonstrated the effectiveness of costly signals of need by females, leaving open the question of the effectiveness of costly signals of need by males.

Compared to the US sample, far more Indian participants failed our attention checks, which is consistent with botting, unfamiliarity with English, or low-effort responses (Kennedy et al., 2020). If this high failure rate indicates lower-quality responses among those who passed the attention checks, the weak signal effect in the thwarted marriage vignette may simply be due to greater noise rather than differences in the scenario or the effectiveness of the signals compared to those in the US. Another concern relevant to all vignettes is that our decision to anchor the T1 sliders at 0 may have resulted in participants being more likely to report extreme values.

Finally, we adopted game theory models of bargaining with incomplete information as our theoretical framework, but there are many other models of credible signaling (e.g., Számadó, 2011), including for need (Számadó, Czégel, & Zachar, 2019) and suicidality (Rosenthal, 1993). If depression and suicidality involve signaling, they might be better explained by a different model.

Facial width (not width relative to height) may be a key to facial sex differences (appears linked in men to other possibly sexually-selected traits)

Caton, Neil R., and Barnaby Dixson. 2022. “Beyond Facial Width-to-height Ratios: Bizygomatic Width Is Highly Sexually Dimorphic When Adjusting for Allometry.” PsyArXiv. February 21. doi:10.31234/osf.io/g5rwm

Abstract: A large literature implicates male facial width-to-height ratio (bizygomatic width divided by facial height) as a secondary sexual trait linked to numerous physical and psychological outcomes. However, this research is based entirely on the premise that bizygomatic width is sexually dimorphic, which recent research has called this into question. Unfortunately, statisticians for the last 125 years have noted that ratio measurements engender spurious correlations and biased effect-size estimates. In the current study, we find that bizygomatic width is highly sexually dimorphic (equivalent d = 1.39). Further, after adjusting for 92 allometric measurements, including multiple facial height and other craniofacial measurements, bizygomatic width exhibited pronounced male-biased sexual dimorphism (equivalent d = 1.01) in a sample of 6,068 men and women born across the globe (Europe, Asia, Oceania, North, Central, and South America). In contrast, fWHR measurements demonstrated a statistical pattern consistent with the age-old argument that ratio measurements engender spurious correlations and biased effect-size estimates. Thus, when avoiding ratios and adjusting for allometry in craniofacial measures, we found strong support for a key premise in the human evolutionary and behavioral sciences that bizygomatic width exhibits male-biased sexual dimorphism.


Explanations of misfortune: Themes include intervention of superhuman agents (gods, ancestors), witchcraft, karma; seen in evolutionary context, members are required to offer support, willing to offer such support to maintain reputation as cooperators

Why we blame victims, accuse witches, invent taboos and invoke spirits: A model of strategic responses to misfortune. Pascal Boyer. Forthcoming, 2021, Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute. pascalboyer.net/articles/2021-Boyer-Misfortune.pdf

Abstract. Explanations of misfortune are the object of much cultural discourse in most human societies. Recurrent themes include the intervention of superhuman agents (gods, ancestors, etc.), witchcraft, karma, and the violation of specific rules or “taboos”. In modern large-scale societies, people often respond by blaming the victims of, e.g., accidents and assault. These responses may seem both disparate and puzzling, in the sense that the proposed accounts of untoward events provide no valuable information about their causes or the best way to prevent them. However, these responses make sense if we see them in an evolutionary context, where accidents, assault and illness were common occurrences, the only palliative being social support to victims. This would create a context in which all members of a group may be a) required to offer support, b) willing to offer such support to maintain a reputation as cooperators, and c) desirous to limit that support because of its cost. In this context, recurrent explanations of misfortune would constitute strategic attempts to create and broadcast a specific description of the situation that concentrates responsibility and potential costs on a few individuals. This strategic model accounts for otherwise puzzling features of explanations based on mystical harm (ancestors, witchcraft, etc.), as well as the tendency to denigrate victims, and offers new predictions about those cultural phenomena. 


Across 47 societies (statistically controlling for wealth), a societal emphasis on socializing children for religious faith attenuates links of personal religiosity with happiness, trust of strangers, and trust of known others

Societal Emphasis on Religious Faith as a Cultural Context for Shaping the Social-Psychological Relationships Between Personal Religiosity and Well-Being. Liman Man Wai Li, Xiaobin Lou, Michael Harris Bond. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, February 22, 2022. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221079875

Abstract: How does a society’s religious context affect the relationships between personal religiosity and well-being? To explore this question, we used two measures of personal religiosity, the absolute importance of religion, and the importance of religion relative to the importance of six life domains, viz., family, friends, work, politics, leisure, and religion. To test the generalizability of relationships between these two measures of personal religiosity and well-being, we tested them across representative samples of 66,992 persons from 47 societies varying in their emphasis on socializing children for religious faith. Pan-societally, personal religiosity predicted many of the five well-being measures including satisfaction with life, happiness, subjective health, trust of strangers, and trust of known others, but in opposite directions depending on whether the absolute or the relative importance of personal religiosity was used. Controlling for wealth, a societal emphasis on socializing children for religious faith moderated the links of personal religiosity with happiness, trust of strangers, and trust of known others, but most evidence revealed that a societal emphasis on religious faith attenuated the strength of these linkages. We argue that measuring an individual’s religiosity in the context of their daily living yields a more realistic view of religion’s role in personal life and social living and suggest that there are both personal and social costs for investing strongly in religion relative to other domains of daily life. Societal religious context must also be assessed to provide a more nuanced understanding of personal religiosity and its associated correlates.

Keywords: personal religiosity, subjective well-being, trust, societal priorities for socializing children


Saturday, February 26, 2022

Endogenous androgens show a moderate association with a better sexual function in women; however, the role of psychological, relational and other hormonal factors should not be overlooked

Maseroli E and Vignozzi L. Are Endogenous Androgens Linked to Female Sexual Function? A Systemic Review and Meta-Analysis. J Sex Med 2021;XX:XXX–XXX. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2022.01.515

Abstract

Background: The benefits of treatment with testosterone (T) in women with loss of desire suggest that low androgens may distinguish women with sexual dysfunction (SD) from others; however, evidence on this point is lacking.

Aim: To answer the question: is there an association between endogenous levels of androgens and sexual function in women?

Methods: An extensive search was performed in MEDLINE, Embase and PsycInfo. Four separate meta-analyses were conducted for total T, free T, Free Androgen Index (FAI), and Dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate (DHEAS). Cohort, cross-sectional, and prospective studies were included.

Outcomes: The main outcome was the association between endogenous androgens and sexual desire. Global sexual function was considered as a secondary outcome. The effect measure was expressed as standardized mean difference (SMD).

Results: The meta-analysis on total T included 34 studies involving 3,268 women, mean age 36.5 years. In 11 studies, a significant association was found between sexual desire, measured by validated psychometric instruments, and total T (SMD = 0.59 [0.29;0.88], P < 0.0001), with a moderate effect. The association with global sexual function (n = 12 studies) was also significant (SMD = 0.44 [0.21;0.67], P <0.0001). Overall, total T was associated with a better sexual function (SMD = 0.55 [0.28;0.82)], P < 0.0001), with similar results obtained when poor quality studies were removed. Age showed a negative relationship with the overall outcome. No differences were found when stratifying the studies according to menopausal status, type of menopause, age at menopause, use of hormonal replacement therapy, relationship status, method for T measurement, phase of the menstrual cycle or use of hormonal contraception. The meta-analysis of T derivatives (free T and FAI) also showed a significant, moderate association with sexual desire. In contrast, DHEAS seems not to exert any significant influence on desire, whilst showing a positive association with global sexual function.

Clinical Implications: Endogenous androgens show a moderate association with a better sexual function in women; however, the role of psychological, relational and other hormonal factors should not be overlooked.

Strengths & Limitations: This represents the first attempt at meta-analyzing data available on the topic. A significant publication bias was found for total T.

Conclusion: There appears to be a moderate association between total T and sexual desire/global sexual function, which is confirmed, although weak, in studies employing liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry (LC-MS). Similar results on desire were obtained for free T and FAI. DHEAS only showed a positive association with global sexual function. More research is needed.

Key Words: TestosteroneHSDDDHEASAndrogensFemale Sexual Dysfunction


In anthropomorphic terms we could say that the female rat invites the male to copulate, i.e. she expresses her consent to having sex, and the male can accept or not accept that invitation by pursuing or not pursuing the female

Sexual incentive motivation, sexual behavior, and general arousal: Do rats and humans tell the same story? Anders Ågmo, Ellen Laan. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, February 26 2022, 104595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104595

Highlights

• Sexual motivation is activated by external stimuli called sexual incentives

• Exposure to sexual incentives leads also to enhanced general arousal

• General arousal enhances activity in the sexual central motive state

• Activity in the sexual central motive state requires the presence of gonadal hormones

• The determinants of interindividual differences in sexual motivation are unclear

Abstract: Sexual incentive stimuli activate sexual motivation and heighten the level of general arousal. The sexual motive may induce the individual to approach the incentive, and eventually to initiate sexual acts. Both approach and the ensuing copulatory interaction further enhance general arousal. We present data from rodents and humans in support of these assertions. We then suggest that orgasm is experienced when the combined level of excitation surpasses a threshold. In order to analyze the neurobiological bases of sexual motivation, we employ the concept of a central motive state. We then discuss the mechanisms involved in the long- and short-term control of that state as well as those mediating the momentaneous actions of sexual incentive stimuli. This leads to an analysis of the neurobiology behind the interindividual differences in responsivity of the sexual central motive state. Knowledge is still fragmentary, and many contradictory observations have been made. Nevertheless, we conclude that the basic mechanisms of sexual motivation and the role of general arousal are similar in rodents and humans.

Keywords: Sexual motivationgeneral arousalcentral motive stategonadal hormonespituitary hormoneshypothalamic hormonestransmittersdopamineorgasmgenital arousal


5.3. Viscerosomatic responses and general arousal

Visceral responses, mainly genital blood flow, to sexual stimuli have been the subject of much research. It is possible that these responses are associated with enhanced general arousal. This could easily be shown by calculating correlations between the magnitude of the genital response (erection and vaginal lubrication) and typical indicators of general arousal such as blood pressure, heart rate, respiration frequency or the galvanic skin response. Although all these variables have been recorded in the same individuals in many studies (e.g. Suschinsky and Lalumière, 2012), reports of correlations are exceptional. In fact, there is only one study in which correlations between the genital response and heart rate and the galvanic skin response were calculated (Laan et al., 1995). Contrary to what could be expected, both vaginal pulse amplitude and vaginal blood volume during exposure to a pornographic movie segment were unrelated to heart rate and the skin response. This observation suggest that the intensity of genital arousal is not a main determinant of general arousal in women. The possible relationship between the intensity of the viscerosomatic response of erection and the level of general arousal has not been studied.

Whereas the effects of genital responses on general arousal is unclear, there are many observations regarding the inverse relationship, i.e. the effects of general arousal on genital responses. In fact, there is a reasonable amount of data available from studies that have employed objective measures of genital responses to explicit sexual stimuli after manipulations of the level of general arousal. In an elegant experiment by Cindy Meston (Meston, 2000) women were shown a sexual stimulus (pornographic movie segment) either when rested or after intense physical exercise. It is well known that exercise increases general arousal. The vaginal response to the movie segment was enhanced in the women having engaged in physical exercise, i.e. women with enhanced general arousal. Similar effects were found after treatment with ephedrine, an agonist at postsynaptic adrenergic α and β receptors (Meston and Heiman, 1998). Stimulation of these receptors enhances activity in the sympathetic nervous system, another arousal-producing event. Anxiety has also been found to increase the vaginal response to sexual stimuli (Hoon et al., 1977Palace and Gorzalka, 1990), and anxiety is thought to enhance arousal (Ramsey, 1943). Studies in men have also shown that enhanced general arousal, because of conditioned fear, leads to enhanced penile response to a sexually relevant stimulus (Barlow et al., 1983). Likewise, an emotionally arousing, non-sexual video fragment enhances the penile response to an ensuing pornographic video segment (Wolchik et al., 1980). Since enhanced response to a constant stimulus must be due to increased motivation, it can tentatively be concluded that increasing general arousal stimulates the activity of the sexual central motive state, hence sexual motivation, in men and women.

Although there are many studies supporting the conclusion above, it must be mentioned that an extensive review of the effects of anxiety on sexual responses found that the relationship may be more complicated (e.g. Bradford and Meston, 2006). Several studies have failed to find any effect at all of anxiety on genital responses (e.g. Elliott & O'Donohue, 1997Sipski et al., 2004) while others have reported decreased response (Masters & Johnson, 1970). Consequently, it is premature to make any firm affirmation concerning the influence of general arousal on genital responses in men and women.

Pupil dilation is a non-sexual response to many kinds of stimuli (reviewed in Einhäuser, 2017). When recorded during exposure to a sexual stimulus, it might be considered a visceral response to this stimulus. If young men and women are stressed and concurrently exposed to pictures with erotic content, the pupil dilation are larger than in the absence of stress, and the stress effect is larger for erotic pictures than for neutral pictures (Finke et al., 2018). The stress-induction procedure was found to increase blood pressure and heart rate, showing that general arousal indeed was enhanced. This study suggests that general arousal not only enhances sexual visceral responses to sexual stimuli, but also non-sexual visceral responses to such stimuli. We propose that this action is mediated by the sexual central motive state, and that the heightened pupillary response is indicative of facilitated cognitive processing of sexual stimuli.

The role of general arousal for the viscerosomatic responses in rodents is poorly known. Only one study has evaluated the consequences of enhanced general arousal for penile erection. Rats were subjected to an anxiety-inducing procedure, and erections stimulated by apomorphine were quantified. The anxiety procedure enhanced heart rate and mean arterial pressure, confirming increased arousal. The number of erections observed in these animals was reduced compared to controls (Brien et al., 2002). Even if these data are not conclusive, they suggest that there might be a negative relationship between increased general arousal and erection. Nevertheless, it is probably prudent to abstain from any conclusion until more data have become available.

A curious illustration of a possible relationship between general arousal and sexual responses is found in the association between yawning and penile erection in male rats. Prolonged observation has revealed that erection and yawning often coincides (Holmgren et al., 1985). A major function of yawning is to enhance general arousal in environments providing little stimulation (Baenninger, 1997), such as a rat’s home cage. The momentarily enhanced arousal leads to a genital response, suggesting activation of the central motive state. Whether the association between yawning and sexual activation also is present in humans is not known. Regardless of this, there are probably other, equally likely, explanations for the temporal coincidence of yawning and erection.

We know of no data concerning vaginal responses and general arousal in rodents. In the absence of more experimental evidence in favor of effects of general arousal on genital responses in males and females we propose that rats are different from humans in the way that variations in general arousal have variable effects on genital responses to sexual incentives.

There is an indirect way in which general arousal could affect human genital responses. The level of arousal is known to be a determinant of the efficiency of cognitive processing, in the way that moderate arousal facilitates processing whereas very low or very high arousal have deleterious effects (e.g. Lambourne and Tomporowski, 2010). The importance of cognitive processes for genital responses in men and women has been well established (Tavares et al., 2020). Interference with cognitive processing of sexual stimuli leads to reduced genital response. For example, while women were watching a pornographic movie, they were asked to perform tasks of increasing difficulty. The lowest level of difficulty were no task, and the highest level was a requirement to classify pairs of digits according to complex rules, and verbalize the result aloud (Salemink and van Lankveld, 2006). The classification rules were complex indeed: together the digits form a number; odd numbers under 50 are classified as A; even numbers under 50 are classified as B; odd numbers over 50 are classified as B; even numbers over 50 are classified as A (Salemink and van Lankveld, 2006, p. 182). The more difficult the task, the larger the reduction of the genital response. Similar data have been reported from a male sample (van Lankveld & van den Hout, 2004). Manipulations facilitating attention to sexual stimuli enhanced responses in men and women (Both et al., 2011Farkas et al., 1979). Perhaps the somewhat variable effects of arousal-enhancing procedures on human genital responses can be explained by their unintended, equally variable, effects on cognitive processing.

5.4. Transition from approach to copulation

In humans, the transition involves a conscious decision about whether to proceed with sexual activities or not to proceed. Decision-making is known to enhance general arousal (Johnson, 1963) as manifested in increased heart rate and galvanic skin response. Likewise, pupil dilation, an exquisite indicator of general arousal (Bradley et al., 2008) occurs during the decision process (de Gee et al. (2014) and references therein). It seems likely that the decision to pass from sexual approach behaviors to sexual activity leads to enhanced general arousal as other decisions do. The same should be the case for the contrary decision, not to proceed with sex. Even though the cognitive processes underlying the decision to engage in sexual activities are poorly known, some informed speculation is possible.

It seems that most humans will not initiate partnered sex without explicit or implicit consent from the partner. In fact, in Western societies, and probably in many others, sexual approaches and ensuing sexual activities are socially acceptable only between consenting individuals. Predictions about obtaining consent for sexual interaction are probably an important part of the initial transition from approach to further sexual activity. However important such predictions may be at early stages of sexual approach, these predictions must be confirmed before manifest sexual activities start. Thus, the approaching individual needs to determine whether the approached individual is consenting to sexual activity or not. There seems to have existed conflicting views on how consent or lack thereof is communicated to a potential partner. It now appears that the consenting individual uses direct or indirect verbal expressions as well as direct and indirect non-verbal cues to communicate consent (reviewed in Fenner, 2017). These strategies are also used in pornographic movies (Willis et al., 2020), which may be important since, for many young people, such movies have become the main source of sex education.

Men and women communicate consent in similar ways, and both sexes understand their partner’s communication equally well (Hickman and Muehlenhard, 1999), even in casual encounters (Beres, 2010). The fact that men accused of rape or sexual harassment frequently argue that they misunderstood the message from the non-consenting party can be considered a kind of unfounded self-justification (Maruna and Mann, 2006).

Provided consent is obtained, direct physical interaction between the approaching and approached individuals will usually ensue. This will further enhance general arousal (see below). The contrary outcome, the potential partner’s refusal of consent, may also lead to enhanced arousal, perhaps because of frustration. Even the act of refusing further interaction may lead to enhanced arousal. There is some indirect evidence for this (Chen et al., 2014Zhou et al., 2009). Thus, whatever the outcome, the transition process probably enhances general arousal, and consequently contributes to the activity in the central motive state. Possible consequences are that the rejected individual persists in approach behaviors even if the partner has declined, or that these behaviors become directed towards another individual. There is, however, no direct evidence for any of these alternatives. Enhanced arousal in the rejecting individual will probably reinforce some central motive state different from the sexual.

In rodents, the search for consent before initiating copulation does not seem to be of major concern. One reason for this may be that rodents are not sexually attracted to other individuals unless these others show unequivocal signs of being ready for copulation. We have carefully analyzed the sexual interactions between male and female rats in a seminatural environment, and we have found that sexually experienced males rarely approach a non-receptive female (Chu and Ågmo, 2015aChu and Ågmo, 2015bLe Moëne et al., 2020a). An example is shown in Fig. 3. Furthermore, a male almost never mount a female not showing paracopulatory behavior (Bergheim et al., 2015). Thus, the execution of copulatory acts on part of the male depends on the female’s behavior. Likewise, males need almost always to pursue the female before they come into position for executing a mount. In anthropomorphic terms we could say that the female rat invites the male to copulate, i.e. she expresses her consent to having sex, and the male can accept or not accept that invitation by pursuing or not pursuing the female. It appears, then, that sexual interactions among rats are limited to consensual sexual activities. This is not true in standard observation procedures, in which the experimenter subjects the rats to forced sexual interaction, but it is true in ecologically valid procedures, like the seminatural environment. In any case, whereas the transition from approach to copulation in humans is based on a conscious decision, there is no reason to believe that something similar occurs in rats. There, the transition appears to be an automatic process. Thus, there is no arousal-enhancing decision making involved.

[...]

7. Sexual and general arousal are intrinsically associated

In the preceding overview, we have tried to ascertain the potential role of several compounds in the long- and short-term control of sexual motivation, i.e. the activity of the sexual central motive state. Furthermore, we have described how these compounds may be affected by general arousal. One purpose was to find out whether there is a bidirectional relationship between actions on sexual motivation and actions on general arousal. Table 2 summarizes the results of this analysis. It appears that there is not any necessary coincidence between release caused by general arousal and effects on the central motive state neither in rodents nor in humans. Since data concerning the effects of altered general arousal on release of many of the compounds listed in Table 2 are lacking, this is a tentative conclusion. It may also be observed that the compounds known to be released during sex are also released by enhanced general arousal. This is not surprising, since sex enhances general arousal.

We have presented abundant evidence showing that whenever sexual responses are activated, general arousal is also increased. We have also shown that some compounds released during the exposure to sexual incentives simultaneously enhance sexual and general arousal. This coincidence is not surprising. In rats and humans, copulatory behavior involves substantial physical activity, in the form of coordinated activity in skeletal muscles. The level of general arousal is an important determinant of that activity. At the same time, copulation requires a series of specific genital responses. The main genital response in males is erection, in the human an entirely vascular response. In the rat, vascular erection is reinforced by contraction of the striated penile muscles. Furthermore, in the rat, activity in these muscles is necessary for achieving vaginal penetration, and they will also contribute to the expulsion of semen at ejaculation in rats and men. During the first phase of ejaculation, seminal emission, smooth muscles in the epididymis and vas deferens will propel spermatozoa into the urethra while other smooth muscle cells will expel the contents of the seminal vesicle and the prostate. These contents will form the bulk of the ejaculate. In females, the main genital responses are clitoral engorgement and vaginal lubrication. Interestingly, there is some evidence suggesting that these sexual responses may be activated outside of sexual contexts by extreme levels of general arousal (Sachs, 2007Sachs, 2008). What we could call “effective sexual motivation”, or momentaneous activity level in the sexual central motive state, is always a combination of sexual and general arousal, as we try to illustrate in Fig. 1. We have found no evidence for any difference between humans and rodents in this respect.

8. Are the differences between rats and humans more than trivial?

Another purpose with the description of the hormonal and transmitter responses to sexual stimuli or sexual activities or changes in the level of general arousal was to compare rodents and humans. Similarities would strengthen the popular notion that rodents can be used to predict effects of drugs on human sexual functions (Ågmo, 2014Ågmo et al., 2004Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019). Important dissimilarities would question that notion. The summaries presented in Table 1Table 2 clearly suggest a strong similarity between rodents and humans. The neurochemical control of the sexual central motive state seems to have been highly conserved among mammals, and it should, consequently, be possible to make reliable predictions from one mammalian species to another.


A large majority of the population would respond positively to the discovery of microbial extraterrestrial life, independent of factors such as personality or political orientation

Kwon, Jung Yul, Hannah Bercovici, Katja Cunningham, and Michael E. W. Varnum, PhD. 2017. “How Will We React to the Discovery of Extraterrestrial Life?.” OSF Preprints. November 9. doi:10.31219/osf.io/rb5mj

Abstract: How will humanity react to the discovery of extraterrestrial life? Speculation on this topic abounds, but empirical research is practically non-existent. We report the results of three empirical studies assessing psychological reactions to the discovery of extraterrestrial life using the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) text analysis software. We examined language use in media coverage of past discovery announcements of this nature, with a focus on extraterrestrial microbial life (Pilot Study). A large online sample (N = 501) was asked to write about their own and humanity’s reaction to a hypothetical announcement of such a discovery (Study 1), and an independent, large online sample (N = 256) was asked to read and respond to a newspaper story about the claim that fossilized extraterrestrial microbial life had been found in a meteorite of Martian origin (Study 2). Across these studies, we found that reactions were significantly more positive than negative, and more reward vs. risk oriented. A mini- meta-analysis revealed large overall effect sizes (positive vs. negative affect language: g = .98; reward vs. risk language: g = .81). We also found that people’s forecasts of their own reactions showed a greater positivity bias than their forecasts of humanity’s reactions (Study 1), and that responses to reading an actual announcement of the discovery of extraterrestrial microbial life showed a greater positivity bias than responses to reading an actual announcement of the creation of man-made synthetic life (Study 2). Taken together, this work suggests that our reactions to a future confirmed discovery of microbial extraterrestrial life are likely to be fairly positive.


When advising others, participants favored deontological options more, recommending these options over the more utilitarian options that participants chose for themselves

Polman, E., & Ruttan, R. L. (2022). Making utilitarian choices but giving deontological advice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Feb 2022. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001194

Abstract: The traditional focus in the moral decision-making literature has been on understanding when and why people choose a utilitarian option versus a deontological option. However, we suggest that when deciding between these two options, people prefer a third option: to seek out others’ advice—which raises the question, what advice do people give others who are faced with a moral dilemma? In a meta-analysis of responses to 50 unique moral dilemmas, furnished by undergraduates, online panelists, and passers-by, we compared 8,696 self-choice responses (from participants who decided what option they choose) with 8,548 advisor responses (from participants who recommended what option others should choose). We found that when advising others, participants favored deontological options more, recommending these options over the more utilitarian options that participants chose for themselves (d = .112). Our research shows that when people seek advice from others, the two cents they receive are a deontological sense. 


Impact of a sudden and unexpected nation-wide alcohol sales ban in South Africa: This causally reduced injury-induced mortality in the country by at least 14% during the five weeks of the ban; also found a sharp drop in violent crimes

Alcohol, Violence and Injury-Induced Mortality: Evidence from a Modern-Day Prohibition. Barron, Kai / Parry, Charles D.H. / Bradshaw, Debbie / Dorrington, Rob / Groenewald, Pam / Laubscher, Ria / Matzopoulos, Richard. CESifo Working Paper No. 9595. https://www.cesifo.org/en/publikationen/2022/working-paper/alcohol-violence-and-injury-induced-mortality-evidence-modern-day

Abstract: This paper evaluates the impact of a sudden and unexpected nation-wide alcohol sales ban in South Africa. We find that this policy causally reduced injury-induced mortality in the country by at least 14% during the five weeks of the ban. We argue that this estimate constitutes a lower bound on the true impact of alcohol on injury-induced mortality. We also document a sharp drop in violent crimes, indicating a tight link between alcohol and aggressive behaviour in society. Our results underscore the severe harm that alcohol can cause and point towards a role for policy measures that target the heaviest drinkers in society. 

Keywords: alcohol, mortality, economics, health, crime, South Africa, Covid-19, violence

JEL: I180, I120, K420


In academic environments: Evidence that pay transparency causes significant increases in both the equity and equality of pay, & significant and sizeable reductions in the link between pay & individually measured performance

The influence of pay transparency on (gender) inequity, inequality and the performance basis of pay. Tomasz Obloj & Todd Zenger. Nature Human Behaviour, Feb 10 2022. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-022-01288-9

Abstract: Recent decades have witnessed a growing focus on two distinct income patterns: persistent pay inequity, particularly a gender pay gap, and growing pay inequality. Pay transparency is widely advanced as a remedy for both. Yet we know little about the systemic influence of this policy on the evolution of pay practices within organizations. To address this void, we assemble a dataset combining detailed performance, demographic and salary data for approximately 100,000 US academics between 1997 and 2017. We then exploit staggered shocks to wage transparency to explore how this change reshapes pay practices. We find evidence that pay transparency causes significant increases in both the equity and equality of pay, and significant and sizeable reductions in the link between pay and individually measured performance.

Friday, February 25, 2022

Liberals perceive the stock market as a more dangerous and riskier place than conservatives, which explains liberals’ greater support for regulation of the stock market, and their greater opposition to privatization of Social Security

Fear and Loathing of Wall Street: Political Liberalism, Uncertainty, and Threat Management in a Dangerous Economic World. Michael Edem Fiagbenu,Thomas Kessler. Political Psychology, February 25 2022. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12805

Abstract: Conservatives perceive the world as a more dangerous and threatening place than liberals, which explains conservatives’ more cautious social behaviors and their greater support for policies (e.g., anti-immigration and harsher punitive measures) that aim to manage and reduce perceived threats and uncertainties. However, past research operationalized the “world” as a place replete with social and physical threats (e.g., street crimes and terrorism). Less attention has been given to the economic world, which is equally characterized by threatening events (e.g., corporate crimes, stock-market crashes). In four studies, we examined whether differences in appraisal of the economic world explain ideological differences in stock-market participation and neoliberal economic policy preferences. The findings reveal that liberals perceive the stock market as a more dangerous and riskier place than conservatives. This asymmetry explains liberals’ cautious investment behaviors, their greater support for regulation of the stock market, and their greater opposition to privatization of Social Security. We argue that liberals’ greater support for these policies serves to protect investors and the general public from the perceived harms of the economic world. Our findings suggest that the psychological processes underlying threat management and uncertainty reduction are similar for conservatives and liberals, but these processes are context dependent.

Check also Of deadly beans and risky stocks: Political ideology and attitude formation via exploration depend on the nature of the attitude stimuli. Michael Edem Fiagbenu, Jutta Proch, Thomas Kessler. British Journal of Psychology, November 14 2019. https://www.bipartisanalliance.com/2019/11/conservatives-do-not-generally-form.html


---

Sensitivity to Perceived Dangers in the Social and Economic World

The context- and content-dependent nature of the relationship between ideology and threat sensitivity can provide further insights into how conservatives perceive and react to different types of crimes. Here, we distinguish between two psychological contexts: the social world where social and interpersonal interactions occur and the economic world where interactions with corporations, financial markets, etc., transpire. Studies based on the Belief in a Dangerous World scale (Duckitt et al., 2002, p. 92)—a common measure used to assess beliefs about the uncertainties and dangers of social life—have consistently shown that conservatives perceive the social world to be a dangerous place where immoral people aim to rob, assault, and murder law-abiding citizens. This characterization suggests that the social world is a place replete with street crimes and terror (e.g., muggings, burglary, murder) and that conservative’s beliefs about the social world may be shaped by their greater sensitivity to physically harmful crimes (see Crawford, 2017; Eadeh & Chang, 2020). In contrast, white-collar and corporate crimes are widespread in the economic world (e.g., Ponzi schemes, bribery, embezzlements), and liberals are more concerned about these crimes and perceive them as more serious than conservatives (Kroska et al., 2019; Unnerver et al., 2008; Zimring & Hawkins, 1978). But it is unclear whether these differences shape liberals’ perception of the economic world as a more dangerous and threatening place.

Most street crimes including burglaries cause immediate bodily harm (Kopp, 2019). Even in cases when burglaries occur without physical harm, the resultant feeling of victimization triggers cognitive and emotional states similar to those experienced following physical assaults (Janoff-Bulman & Frieze, 1983). It is, indeed, true that street-crime victimization may sometimes create negative economic impacts such as replacement of stolen property, paying medical bills, or unemployment due to injury. However, one may argue that fears and concerns about street crime are probably determined by the perceived risks of physical victimization (proximal effects) rather than economic victimization (distal effects). By extension, ideological differences in perception of the social world as a dangerous place (Jöckel & Früh, 2016; van Leeuwen & Park, 2009) may reflect conservatives’ (vs. liberals’) greater sensitivity to physical threats (see also Napier et al., 2018).

In contrast, white-collar crimes commonly occur during interactions with the economic world. These crimes are sometimes perceived as more socially and economically harmful than street crimes (Cohen, 2016; Friedrichs, 2010; Lynch & Stretesky, 2001; Michel, 2015; Piquero, 2018). The socioeconomic harms created by white-collar crimes also differ qualitatively from street crimes. For example, unethical corporate practices sometimes lead to bankruptcies, market failures, and financial crises (Greenglass et al., 2014; Schoen, 2016; Shover & Grabosky, 2010). These negative events typically cause widespread economic harms (e.g., debt, unemployment, foreclosures, etc.), which endanger livelihoods (Rheinhart & Rogoff, 2009; Yilmazer et al., 2015). Corporate scandals and financial crises weaken trust in the economic world and increase fear and risk perception of financial markets, which in turn reduce participation in the stock market (Giannetti & Wang, 2016; Kuvvet, 2018; Lim & Kim, 2018; Nguyen et al., 2017; Zhou, 2020).

Although economic adversity may create secondary outcomes such as illness, injury, or even suicides (Chang et al., 2013; Lynch & Stretesky, 2001; Michel, 2015; Yilmazer et al., 2015), these distal physical outcomes are usually mediated by proximate economic victimization and moderated by socioeconomic status, preexisting physical or mental health, or even seasonal timing (Ballester et al., 2019; Haw et al., 2014; Margerison-Zilko et al., 2016). Consequently, it can be argued that people’s sensitivity to white-collar crimes is primarily driven by their perceived risks of economic rather than physical victimization. If correct, then liberals’ greater concerns about white-collar crimes suggests that they may be more sensitive to economic victimization than conservatives, which may likely cause liberals (vs. conservatives) to perceive the economic world as a dangerous and threatening place.

Threat Management and Uncertainty Reduction in the Social and Economic World

Since conservatives and liberals are sensitive to different types of perceived threats and dangers, one would expect that they would adopt unique behavioral coping strategies and embrace distinct public policies that are well-suited for threat management and uncertainty reduction. Relative to liberals, conservatives typically amend their lifestyles and restrict their social activities (e.g., reduce traveling, avoid crowds and unsafe places) to safe spaces to minimize their risk of physical victimization (Reinhart, 2017; Sloan et al., 2020). Conservatives also support harsher punitive policies (McCann, 2008), more government spending on street-crime prevention (Rebovich et al., 2000; Ren et al., 2008), and stringent anti-immigration procedures than liberals (Doosje et al., 2009; Stewart et al., 2019). The overarching aims of these policies are to resist perceived harmful social changes and regulate activities that are perceived to increase social disorder.

In contrast, liberals also engage in behaviors which may help to minimize economic threats. Liberals are more cautious and intolerant of financial uncertainties in real and hypothetical stock-market games and also form more negative attitudes towards stocks than conservatives (Fiagbenu et al., 2021a; Kaustia & Torstila, 2011; Moore et al., 2010). Although there appears to the an ideological asymmery in stock ownership, the effect sizes are, however, small or insignificant, (r = .01–.20); and the relationship is moderated by financial self-efficacy (Han et al., 2019). However, these studies, taken together, imply that relative to conservatives, liberals may avoid the stock market presumably because they perceive it as a threatening place. But empirical evidence in support of this assertion remains to be established.

Furthermore, liberals also adopt policies that help them to cope with economic threats. First, experimental manipulations that increase the salience of corporate scandals increase support for regulation of financial institutions (Eadeh & Chang, 2020). In addition, liberals are more likely to support punishment of white-collar crimes and call for more government spending on their prevention than conservatives (Kroska et al., 2019; Michel et al., 2014; Rebovich et al., 2000). Further, liberals are more likely to support tighter regulation of the stock market and corporations than conservatives (Potrafke, 2009; Unnerver et al., 2008). Finally, to protect low-income workers and other vulnerable groups, liberals (vs. conservatives) oppose risky economic policy reforms such as partial privatization of Social Security (Devroye, 2003; Rudolph & Popp, 2009). Thus, relative to conservatives, liberals’ support for these policies can be interpreted as a motivation to reduce the perceived harms caused by unregulated practices in the economic world. If correct, then one would expect that liberals would perceive the economic world as a dangerous and threatening place than conservatives.