Monday, February 17, 2020

Children's understanding of dominance and prestige in China and the UK

Children's understanding of dominance and prestige in China and the UK. Anni Kajanus, Narges Afshordi, Felix Warneken. Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 41, Issue 1, January 2020, Pages 23-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.08.002

Abstract: Individuals can gain high social rank through dominance (based on coercion and fear) and prestige (based on merit and admiration). We conducted a cross-cultural developmental study and tested 5- to 12-year-olds, and adults in the UK and China, aiming to determine (a) the age at which children distinguish dominance and prestige, and (b) the influence of cultural values on rank-related reasoning. We specifically tested participants in China because of the value of prestigious individuals modestly yielding to subordinates, a social skill that becomes more salient with age. In both populations, the distinction between dominance and prestige emerged at five years, and improved over childhood. When reasoning about a resource conflict between a high-ranking party and a subordinate, adults in both countries expected high-rank individuals to win, although Chinese adults were less likely to do so regarding prestigious individuals. Across the two countries, younger children (5–7 years) responded similarly to each other, not favoring either party as the winner. Older children (9–12 years), however, diverged. Those in the UK chose the high-rank party, while those in China made no systematic inference. Overall, our findings suggest that while children distinguish prestige and dominance comparably in the two countries, they develop culturally-influenced expectations about the behavior of high-rank individuals.

5. General discussion

Our aims were to test: (1) the age at which children differentiate dominance and prestige; and (2) the influence of cultural ideas of hierarchy and conflict on expectations about the behaviors of dominant and prestigious individuals. The experiments yielded several key findings for both lines of inquiry.
 
(1)  Developmental trajectory
First, 5- to 12-year-olds in both the UK and China easily identified dominant and prestigious characters as high-ranking. Thus, even for the younger group of children (5–7 years), cues to prestige (e.g. asking for advice, following said advice) were enough to merit inferences about rank. In fact, children were just as successful at recognizing that the prestigious character was higher-ranking as they were for the dominant character. Next, even younger children distinguished between prestigious and dominant characters in a third-party situation, choosing the prestigious character significantly more when asked whom the subordinate would approach or like, than when asked whom she feared. This finding is particularly interesting in light of the fact that children do not shun coercive (or dominance-like) strategies in their peer groups until eight years (Hawley, 1999). Since our younger group was younger than eight years, the finding suggests that shunning coercive strategies is not the result of noticing the difference between dominant and prestigious strategies. Nonetheless, the ability to distinguish dominance and prestige improved with age. Finally, there were no differences between responses in the UK and China, providing some of the first empirical evidence of children from non-Euro-American cultures understanding cues to rank similarly to children in Europe and the US. In summary, children attributed a combination of traits to the characters that are reflective of a conceptual distinction between dominance and prestige, viewing both as having high social rank, but differing in prestigious characters being liked and approached versus dominant individuals being feared and avoided.
One alternative explanation of the findings from Experiment 1 is that children did not actually distinguish dominance from prestige, and instead succeeded in the task by answering the questions (age, liking, approach, fear) piecemeal. In other words, maybe they answered that Dimo would like and approach the prestigious character simply because the character seemed nice and friendly, and feared and avoided the dominant character because that one seemed mean and aggressive. Similarly, children could have inferred age (i.e. rank) by drawing on cues like being imitated. We do not dispute that these cues led children to answer the questions correctly. In fact, we claim that this is exactly what the understanding of prestige and dominance looks like: an understanding of this combination of features. None of these individual features differentiates between dominance and prestige, but one person having high status while being nice and approachable differentiates this person from a similarly high-ranking individual who is mean and aggressive.
Although younger children recognized the rank difference between the characters just as easily as older children (Experiment 1), they did not infer that higher-ranking parties would win resource conflicts (Experiment 3). This failure cannot be attributed to a cultural effect, as children in the UK and China performed similarly. When asked to justify their choice of who would win the conflict, almost no children referenced rank in their explanations, confirming that their failure to infer is a real consequence of how they construed the scene. Thus, although younger children extracted rank from watching interactions between characters in Experiment 1, they were unable to automatically incorporate them into inferences about subsequent behaviors in Experiment 3. Future work should explore this finding further.
 
 
(2)  Cross-cultural differences
A key contribution from our studies is evidence for the influence of cultural norms and value systems on how children and adults understand social hierarchies and reason about them. Adults in the UK and China were similar in that they both inferred that high-ranking characters would win against a subordinate. They did differ, however, in the degree to which they made this inference in the prestige case. Chinese adults were less likely than British adults to think that the prestigious person would win the resource. This difference, while subtle, is a key sign of the cultural difference reflecting the value specifically placed on yielding to others when in a position of prestige (Kajanus, n.d.). The cultural difference also manifested in older children (9–12 years), but in a different way. Older children in the UK inferred that the high-ranking party would win the conflict, regardless of whether the character was prestigious or dominant. In contrast, older children in China responded similarly to younger children in both countries, demonstrating no systematic prediction about who would win in either of the conflict cases. But unlike younger children's explanations, which were shallow and unrelated to social rank, older children in China and the UK provided similar levels of rank-relevant explanations (around 65%). Consequently, the lack of systematic inferences was age-driven in younger children, but culturally influenced in older children in China. Moreover, older children in China who thought the subordinate would win the conflict were more likely to mention the prestigious character yielding than the dominant character yielding. This finding aligns with the value of yielding by prestigious individuals. Together, these findings suggest that the difference between older children in China and the UK is rooted in a cultural difference.
On the topic of Chinese participants' choices, two points merit further discussion. With regard to adults, the fact that Chinese participants thought that both dominant and prestigious parties would win the conflict gives room for pause, given the value of prestigious individuals showing restraint and giving up resources to lower-status parties. However, it is important to note that yielding to those in lower positions is a sophisticated social skill highly dependent on the nuances of the situation, such as the dynamics of ascribed status hierarchies and the importance of the issue at the root of the conflict (Kajanus, n.d.). Thus, most adults may have viewed the situation as one in which the high-ranking character lacked the sophistication to yield, given the cartoonish appearance of the characters, the simplicity of the exchanges, and the prestigious character's unabashed and juvenile desire for the resource. This may have made the high-ranking character's win the more straightforward choice. A number of them may have, nonetheless, considered the situation in terms of the prestigious character yielding, thereby bringing about the difference with the British adults.
Another puzzling finding has to do with the older Chinese children's responses in Experiment 3. In a reverse situation from the adults, children held no expectation of either party winning in either conflict case. The lack of a clear prediction may be understandable for the prestige case, but is more surprising in the dominance case. As the data do not clarify the reason for this result, we can only speculate. One likely account is that children at this age are not yet fully proficient in the complexities of rank relations and the rules of yielding. They have learned that high-ranking persons will sometimes yield, but the ways in which personality characteristics (e.g. prestige, dominance) and ascribed hierarchies (position, age, gender, etc.) factor into yielding are still unclear. It is therefore possible that even though they distinguish between dominant and prestigious processes, in relation to yielding they simply treat them similarly as signs of high rank. It is notable that even adults of both countries treated the prestige and dominance cases the same in their predictions about yielding. This does not mean that they cannot distinguish between prestige and dominance as bases of high rank. Unlike Experiment 1, which specifically tested this ability, Experiments 2 and 3 were focused on predictions about yielding in a conflict between high-ranking and low-ranking character. Even so, Chinese adults were less likely to choose the prestigious character as the winner than the UK adults. Older children also showed signs of making the distinction in relation to yielding, as they were more likely to mention the higher-ranking character yielding in the prestige scene than in the dominance scene.
One limitation of the present studies was the sample size (n = 40 per age, country), which although on par with previous work on children's reasoning about social rank (e.g. Charafeddine et al., 2015, Charafeddine et al., 2016), was not very large. Although a larger sample would have been more beneficial, the number of children in the schools we had access to logistically limited us. We hope to remedy this in our future work.
Relying on large amounts of ethnographic evidence, Fiske (1992) laid out a theory of four elementary forms of social relationships in human societies, one of which is authority ranking, which corresponds to hierarchical relationships marked by rank differences. Even though the relational model of authority ranking might be universal, it can be implemented in myriad ways. Children may be endowed with an innate knowledge of the model of hierarchical relationships (Thomsen & Carey, 2013), but they must learn its details and exactly how it operates in their particular social milieu. These experiments provide evidence for one such culturally-influenced aspect of hierarchical relationships across two cultural backdrops. The range of cultural features and cues to relationships is underexplored, as is how children come to learn them and how long it takes them to do so. We offer an initial glimpse of one particular aspect and hope that future work will go on to uncover many more.

No comments:

Post a Comment