Wednesday, February 19, 2020

Lassitude: Reducing energetically expensive movement to make more energy available to the immune system, reducing exposure to additional infections and injuries, deploying strategies that elicit caregiving behavior from social allies

Lassitude: The emotion of being sick. Joshua M. Schrock, J. Josh Snodgrass, Lawrence S. Sugiyama. Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 41, Issue 1, January 2020, Pages 44-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.09.002

Abstract: Our long co-evolutionary history with infectious agents likely began soon after the rise of the first single-celled organisms. This ongoing evolutionary arms race has generated complex host adaptations, many highly conserved, for resisting infection (e.g., innate and acquired immune systems, infection-sensitive developmental programs, sexual reproduction). A large body of evidence suggests that, in humans, pathogen-avoidance disgust is an emotion that motivates avoidance of cues associated with pathogens, thereby reducing infection. However, the question of whether there is an emotion that coordinates resistance to active infection has received less attention. We propose that lassitude is such an emotion. It is triggered by cues of active infection and coordinates the fight against infection by: (a) reducing energetically expensive movement to make more energy available to the immune system, (b) reducing exposure to additional infections and injuries that would compound the immune system's workload, (c) promoting thermoregulatory behaviors that facilitate immunity, (d) regulating food consumption to be beneficial for the host but detrimental to pathogens, and (e) deploying strategies that elicit caregiving behavior from social allies. Lassitude exhibits the core features of an emotion – it is triggered by cues of an adaptive problem (i.e., infection), generates a characteristic facial expression (e.g., slack facial muscles, drooping eyelids, slightly parted lips), and has distinct qualia (e.g., profound tiredness, reduced appetite, feelings of vulnerability, altered temperature perception, increased pain sensitivity). We outline the information-processing structure of lassitude, review existing evidence, suggest directions for future research, and discuss implications of lassitude for models of human evolution.


5. Lassitude: a new emotion?

Our approach to characterizing lassitude is informed by Tooby and Cosmides' framework: “[the emotions] are the neurocomputational adaptations that have evolved in response to the adaptive problem of matching arrays of mechanism activation to the specific adaptive demands imposed by alternative situations” (Tooby & Cosmides, 2008, p. 117). Lassitude satisfies this definition of an emotion. It is a coordinating system that functions to orchestrate various mechanisms to solve the adaptive problem of fighting infectious disease.
Other theoretical approaches emphasize the distinctive facial expression and qualia of an emotion (Ekman & Oster, 1979; Frijda, 2005). We propose that lassitude has a distinctive facial expression generated by less muscle tension relative to a neutral facial expression (i.e., slack facial muscles). In particular, it consists of a long crown-to-chin length, drooping eyelids, and slightly parted lips. A recent study showed participants pictures of faces of people who had been injected with LPS or with placebo (Axelsson et al., 2018). Participants correctly identified the faces of sick people at a higher rate than chance, even though individuals in the photos were instructed to exhibit a neutral facial expression, which may have resulted in partial masking of the facial expression of lassitude. The faces of sick people were rated as having droopy corners of the mouth, having hanging eyelids, looking more tired, having redder eyes, and having paler, puffier skin. We propose that lassitude also has distinct qualia – profound tiredness, greater relative preference for close social allies, reduced overall appetite but stronger relative preference for particular food items, greater feelings of vulnerability, increased pain sensitivity, greater susceptibility to nausea, and altered perceptions of ambient temperature. Not all of these qualia are present in all cases of infection – the presence of each component depends on context-sensitive regulation of the underlying motivational system.
Panksepp's account of basic emotions emphasizes universality across species of mammals and conserved neural circuitry (Panksepp, 1982). Lassitude involves specific immune and neural signaling pathways that have been mapped with increasing thoroughness in animal models (McCusker & Kelley, 2013). Lassitude-like regulatory states exist not only in mammals but also birds (Owen-Ashley & Wingfield, 2007), amphibians (Llewellyn et al., 2011), and possibly even fish (Kirsten, Fior, Kreutz, & Barcellos, 2018).
Based on the criteria discussed here, lassitude is an emotion, albeit one that has gone unrecognized. It is worth noting that there are at least two ways in which our account of lassitude is not novel. First, Tooby and Cosmides hypothesized that malaise during infection might be an evolved emotion but did not elaborate (Tooby & Cosmides, 2008). Second, scholars who study sickness behavior, focusing mostly on non-humans, have proposed that many infection-induced behavioral changes are evolutionarily adaptive (Adelman & Martin, 2009; Hart, 1990). However, the sickness behavior literature largely focuses on the description of infection-related behavior and its physiological mechanisms. It has largely neglected to characterize the evolutionary background, information-processing structure, and functional logic of the regulatory system that generates sickness behavior.

6. Directions for future research

6.1. Social support and immunity

Humans provide care to social allies during illness and injury (see section 2.2.5). This is an important buffer against the opportunity costs of reducing movement when sick. A compelling hypothesis to explain the placebo effect is that one function of visible illness symptoms is to elicit care from others (Steinkopf, 2015). When others provide care, the signaling function of the symptoms is fulfilled and the symptoms become less severe (ibid). We predict that cues of social support (or a lack thereof) are also key inputs for modulating the regulation of lassitude during infection. Infected individuals who are socially isolated may be unable to afford to devote as much energy to fighting infection, and may therefore experience longer-lasting infections and higher mortality risks. In support of this hypothesis, there is evidence to suggest that those who are socially isolated tend to suffer from poorer health (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2014).

6.2. Lassitude in healthcare settings

We hypothesize that cues of infection increase the relative preference for contact with social allies. Furthermore, we propose that sick individuals boost signals of vulnerability when in the presence of social allies (in order to elicit care) and mask signals of vulnerability in front of strangers and antagonists (in order to reduce the risk of social and physical danger during the vulnerable state of sickness). This suggests that the degree to which a patient sees a healthcare provider as a social ally may have a major influence on a patient's decision to pursue care, and ability to elicit it.
Patients who do not see providers as social allies may be less likely to seek healthcare, and when they do, may have greater difficulty eliciting useful care, due to the fact that they are (perhaps inadvertently) masking signals of vulnerability. This may help explain why many patients highly value good “bedside manner” in healthcare providers (Thompson & Anderson, 1982).

6.3. Lassitude and chronic disease

There is evidence to suggest that chronic diseases (e.g., heart disease, diabetes, chronic obstructive pulmonary disorder) may activate a response that resembles a chronic version of lassitude (Swain, 2000). This may be due, in part, to the fact that chronic somatic damage activates some of the same pro-inflammatory immune pathways that trigger lassitude during infection (Del Giudice & Gangestad, 2018; McCusker & Kelley, 2013). This poses a problem because one of most effective interventions for preventing and treating chronic disease is to engage in healthy levels of physical activity (Warburton, Nicol, & Bredin, 2006). The motivational state of lassitude directly opposes this goal. Thus, even sub-clinical levels of chronic morbidity may trigger a vicious, self-reinforcing cycle in which greater chronic morbidity leads to greater lassitude, and greater lassitude leads to even greater chronic morbidity. This cycle may help explain why chronic disease epidemics emerge when populations transition to economic sectors with a greater proportion of sedentary occupations (Omran, 2005).

6.4. Lassitude in relation to time, reward, and risk preferences

We hypothesize that lassitude modifies the cost-benefit structure of a wide range of decisions. Individuals in a state of lassitude place a lower value on some types of rewards (e.g., food, sex). Higher levels of lassitude may therefore generate a greater willingness to delay some kinds of payoffs in temporal discounting scenarios (Green, Myerson, & Mcfadden, 1997). We also propose that individuals in a state of lassitude place a greater value on avoiding social and physical risks. Thus, lassitude may induce greater risk aversion when the risks are social or physical. On the other hand, lassitude may induce less aversion to the risk of losing a potential payoff that has lower value during lassitude (e.g., food, sex). Researchers who study time-, reward-, and risk-related decision-making should therefore consider incorporating lassitude into their studies and theoretical models.

6.5. Approach-avoidance conflict

The fact that humans systematically help social allies during illness suggests that we have cognitive mechanisms for detecting signs of illness in others and deciding how to respond. The occurence of illness in a social conspecific poses a potential motivational conflict. On one hand, helping the sick individual may induce the target of aid to reciprocate in the future, when the roles are reversed (Gurven et al., 2000; Sugiyama, 2004; Sugiyama & Chacon, 2000). Furthermore, helping sick individuals provides a costly signal of the helper's quality as a social ally (Steinkopf, 2015). On the other hand, the effort required for providing aid is costly, and providing aid to a sick person may increase the helper's risk of getting sick (Tybur et al., 2012). Thus, navigating the motivational conflict between approaching a sick individual (to help them) and avoiding them (to reduce infection risk) is an important adaptive problem for humans. Variables that influence the decision to help or avoid a sick person may include the relationship/kinship of the sick person to the helper, the sick person's reputation as a reciprocator, the helper's vulnerability to illness, and the social audience that would be aware of the helping behavior.

No comments:

Post a Comment