Wednesday, August 5, 2020

The intertemporal cortex of untrained primates can serve as a precursor of orthographic processing; the acquisition of reading in humans seems to rely on the recycling of a brain network evolved for other visual functions

The inferior temporal cortex is a potential cortical precursor of orthographic processing in untrained monkeys. Rishi Rajalingham, Kohitij Kar, Sachi Sanghavi, Stanislas Dehaene & James J. DiCarlo. Nature Communications volume 11, Article number: 3886. Aug 4 2020. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-020-17714-3

Abstract: The ability to recognize written letter strings is foundational to human reading, but the underlying neuronal mechanisms remain largely unknown. Recent behavioral research in baboons suggests that non-human primates may provide an opportunity to investigate this question. We recorded the activity of hundreds of neurons in V4 and the inferior temporal cortex (IT) while naïve macaque monkeys passively viewed images of letters, English words and non-word strings, and tested the capacity of those neuronal representations to support a battery of orthographic processing tasks. We found that simple linear read-outs of IT (but not V4) population responses achieved high performance on all tested tasks, even matching the performance and error patterns of baboons on word classification. These results show that the IT cortex of untrained primates can serve as a precursor of orthographic processing, suggesting that the acquisition of reading in humans relies on the recycling of a brain network evolved for other visual functions.


Discussion
A key goal of human cognitive neuroscience is to understand how the human brain supports the ability to learn to recognize written letters and words. This question has been investigated for several decades using human neuroimaging techniques, yielding putative brain regions that may uniquely underlie orthographic abilities7,8,9. In the work presented here, we sought to investigate this issue in the primate ventral visual stream of naïve rhesus macaque monkeys. Non-human primates such as rhesus macaque monkeys have been essential to study the neuronal mechanisms underlying human visual processing, especially in the domain of object recognition where monkeys and humans exhibit remarkably similar behavior and underlying brain mechanisms, both neuroanatomical and functional13,14,15,16,39,40. Given this strong homology, and the relative recency of reading abilities in the human species, we reasoned that the high-level visual representations in the primate ventral visual stream could serve as a precursor that is recycled by developmental experience for human orthographic processing abilities. In other words, we hypothesized that the neural representations that directly underlie human orthographic processing abilities are strongly constrained by the prior evolution of the primate visual cortex, such that representations present in naïve, illiterate, non-human primates could be minimally adapted to support orthographic processing. Here, we observed that orthographic information was explicitly encoded in sampled populations of spatially distributed IT neurons in naïve, illiterate, non-human primates. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that the population of IT neurons in each subject forms an explicit (i.e., linearly separable, as per ref. 21) representation of orthographic objects, and could serve as a common substrate for learning many visual discrimination tasks, including ones in the domain of orthographic processing.

We tested a battery of 30 orthographic tests, focusing on a word classification task (separating English words from pseudowords). This task is referred to as “lexical decision” when tested on literate subjects recognizing previously learned words (i.e., when referencing a learned lexicon). For nonliterate subjects (e.g., baboons or untrained IT decoders), word classification is the ability to identify orthographic features that distinguish between words and pseudowords and generalize to novel strings. This generalization must rely on specific visual features whose distribution differs between words and pseudowords; previous work suggests that such features may correspond to specific bigrams17, position-specific letter combinations41, or distributed visual features42. While this battery of tasks is not an exhaustive characterization of orthographic processing, we found that it has the power to distinguish between alternative hypotheses. Indeed, these tasks could not be accurately performed by linear readout decoders of the predominant input visual representation to IT (area V4) or by approximations of lower levels of the ventral visual stream, unlike many other coarse discrimination tasks (e.g., contrasting orthographic and nonorthographic stimuli). We note that the successful classifications from IT-based decoders do not necessarily imply that the brain exclusively uses IT or the same coding schemes and algorithms that we have used for decoding. Rather, the existence of this sufficient code in untrained and illiterate non-human primates suggests that the primate ventral visual stream could be minimally adapted through experience-dependent plasticity to support orthographic processing behaviors.

These results are consistent with a variant of the “neuronal recycling” theory, which posits that the features that support visual object recognition may have been coopted for written word recognition5,6,24. Specifically, this variant of the theory is that humans have inherited a pre-existing brain system (here, the ventral visual stream) from recent evolutionary ancestors, and they either inherited or evolved learning mechanisms that enable individuals to adapt the outputs of that system during their lifespan for word recognition and other core aspects of orthographic processing. Consistent with this, our results suggest that prereading children likely have a neural population representation that can readily be reused to learn invariant word recognition. Relatedly, it has been previously proposed that the initial properties of this system may explain the child’s early competence and errors in letter recognition, e.g., explaining why children tend to make left-right inversion errors by the finding that IT neurons tend to respond similarly to horizontal mirror images of objects36,37,43. Consistent with this, we here found that the representation of IT-based decoders exhibited a similar signature of left-right mirror symmetry. According to this proposal, this neural representation would become progressively shaped to support written word recognition in a specific script over the course of reading acquisition, and may also explain why all human writing systems throughout the world rely on a universal repertoire of basic shapes24. As shown in the present work, those visual features are already well encoded in the ventral visual pathway of illiterate primates, and may bias cultural evolution by determining which scripts are more easily recognizable and learnable.

A similar “neuronal recycling hypothesis” has been proposed for the number system: all primates may have inherited a pre-existing brain system (in the intraparietal sulcus) in which approximate number and other quantitative information is well encoded44,45. It has been suggested that these existing representations of numerosity may be adapted to support exact, symbolic arithmetic, and may bias the cultural evolution of numerical symbols6,46. Likewise, such representations have been found to spontaneously emerge in neural network models optimized for other visual functions47. Critically, the term “recycling,” in the narrow sense in which it was introduced, refers to such adaptations of neural mechanisms evolved for evolutionary older functions to support newer cultural functions, where the original function is not entirely lost and the underlying neural functionality constrains what the brain can most easily learn. It remains to be seen whether all instances of developmental plasticity meet this definition, or whether learning may also simply replace unused functions without recycling them48.

In addition to testing a prediction of this neuronal recycling hypothesis, we also explored the question of how orthographic stimuli are encoded in IT neurons. Decades of research has shown that IT neurons exhibit selectivity for complex visual features with remarkable tolerance to changes in viewing conditions (e.g., position, scale, and pose)19,22,23. More recent work demonstrates that the encoding properties of IT neurons, in both humans and monkeys, is best explained by the distributed complex invariant visual features of hierarchical convolutional neural network models30,49,50. Consistent with this prior work, we here found that the firing rate responses of individual neural sites in macaque IT was modulated by, but did not exhibit strong selectivity to orthographic properties, such as letters and letter positions. In other words, we did not observe precise tuning as postulated by “letter detector” neurons, but instead coarse tuning for both letter identity and position. It is possible that, over the course of learning to read, experience-dependent plasticity could fine-tune the representation of IT to reflect the statistics of printed words (e.g., single-neuron tuning for individual letters or bigrams). Moreover, such experience could alter the topographic organization to exhibit millimeter-scale spatial clusters that preferentially respond to orthographic stimuli, as have been shown in juvenile animals in the context of symbol and face recognition behaviors18,51. Together, such putative representational and topographic changes could induce a reorientation of cortical maps towards letters at the expense of other visual object categories, eventually resulting in the specialization observed in the human visual word form area (VWFA). However, our results demonstrate that, even prior to such putative changes, the initial state of IT in untrained monkeys has the capacity to support many learned orthographic discriminations.

In summary, we found that the neural population representation in IT cortex in untrained macaque monkeys is largely able, with some supervised instruction, to extract explicit representations of written letters and words. This did not have to be so—the visual representations that underlie orthographic processing could instead be largely determined over postnatal development by the experience of learning to read. In that case, the IT representation measured in untrained monkeys (or even in illiterate humans) would likely not exhibit the ability to act as a precursor of orthographic processing. Likewise, orthographic processing abilities could have been critically dependent on other brain regions, such as speech and linguistic representations, or putative flexible domain-general learning systems, that evolved well after the evolutionary divergence of humans and Old-World monkeys. Instead, we here report evidence for a precursor of orthographic processing in untrained monkeys. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that learning rests on pre-existing neural representations which it only partially reshapes.

No comments:

Post a Comment