Sunday, September 4, 2022

A machine-predicted brain sex score explains individual differences in cognitive intelligence and genetic influence in young children; sex identified with a 93pct success

The sexual brain, genes, and cognition: A machine-predicted brain sex score explains individual differences in cognitive intelligence and genetic influence in young children. Kakyeong Kim, Yoonjung Yoonie Joo, Gun Ahn, Hee-Hwan Wang, Seo-Yoon Moon, Hyeonjin Kim, Woo-Young Ahn, Jiook Cha. Human Brain Mapping, April 26 2022. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25888


Abstract: Sex impacts the development of the brain and cognition differently across individuals. However, the literature on brain sex dimorphism in humans is mixed. We aim to investigate the biological underpinnings of the individual variability of sexual dimorphism in the brain and its impact on cognitive performance. To this end, we tested whether the individual difference in brain sex would be linked to that in cognitive performance that is influenced by genetic factors in prepubertal children (N = 9,658, ages 9–10 years old; the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study). To capture the interindividual variability of the brain, we estimated the probability of being male or female based on the brain morphometry and connectivity features using machine learning (herein called a brain sex score). The models accurately classified the biological sex with a test ROC–AUC of 93.32%. As a result, a greater brain sex score correlated significantly with greater intelligence (rho-fdr < .001, (eta sub p)**2= .011–.034; adjusted for covariates) and higher cognitive genome-wide polygenic scores (GPSs) (rho-fdr < .001,  (eta sub p)**2 < .005). Structural equation models revealed that the GPS-intelligence association was significantly modulated by the brain sex score, such that a brain with a higher maleness score (or a lower femaleness score) mediated a positive GPS effect on intelligence (indirect effects = .006–.009; p = .002–.022; sex-stratified analysis). The finding of the sex modulatory effect on the gene–brain–cognition relationship presents a likely biological pathway to the individual and sex differences in the brain and cognitive performance in preadolescence.


4 DISCUSSION

We report the novel relationship between brain sex difference, cognitive performance, and shared genetic influence in an admixed American population of prepubertal children. As trained on the grey matter morphometric and white matter connectomes, our machine learning models showed the accurate classification of sex with over 93.32% ROC–AUC in a replication set. Furthermore, the individual variability of the sexual brain development, indexed by the brain-based sex score, showed significant correlations with general intelligence and the inherited genetic influence on general intelligence, the cognitive GPSs. Moreover, the SEM showed that the effect of the cognitive GPSs on cognitive outcomes was modulated by the brain sex score significantly in females and with a similar trend in males. Thus, this study indicates the critical role of brain sex in cognitive performance in prepubertal children, influenced by genetic factors, providing a biological account for the individual variability of neurocognition.

Our study departs from the prior literature on sex differences in intelligence in children by showing the role of the continuum of brain sex on cognitive performance. Literature shows that the group sex differences in mind and behaviors, such as hormonal influences (Vuoksimaa, Kaprio, Eriksson, & Rose, 2012), brain differences (Ostatníková et al., 2010), cultural influences (Penner & Paret, 2008), gender stereotypes (Stoet & Geary, 2012), and biopsychosocial interactions (Haier, Karama, Leyba, & Jung, 2009; Miller & Halpern, 2014). In intelligence, however, literature shows mixed findings of sex differences (Dykiert, Gale, & Deary, 2009). Some show that males have advantages (Irwing & Lynn, 2005; Jackson & Philippe Rushton, 2006; van der Linden, van der Linden, Dunkel, & Madison, 2017) in general intelligence over females, while others show females have advantages over males (Keith, Reynolds, Patel, & Ridley, 2008). These mixed findings may allude to large individual variability in intelligence within sex. Indeed, a recent seminal study shows the biological underpinnings of the individual variability in behavioral phenotypes in adolescents (Vosberg et al., 2020). This study presents an estimate of the continuum of sex based on the brain and body traits, which predicts within each sex the individual variability in sex hormones, personality traits, and internalizing–externalizing behaviors. In line with this, our study further demonstrates the utility of multimodal brain imaging combined with machine learning in estimating an individual status of brain sex. For example, our method permitted the accurate estimation of an individual's developmental status of the brain sex and revealed that the brain sex estimates varied across individuals even within the narrow age range. The discovery of the correlation of the brain sex variability with the genetic and cognitive variables further reflects that this novel estimate may represent a critical neurobiological process.

Another pattern to note is the greater association of crystallized intelligence (the ability that is acquired throughout life: i.e., knowledge, facts, and skills) with the brain-based sex, as well as GPSs for cognitive capacity, compared with fluid intelligence (the ability to reason and solve problems in novel situations; a trend towards significance). These findings are partially in line with prior genetic research showing that crystallized intelligence is greatly associated with genetic influence than fluid intelligence (Christoforou et al., 2014; Genç et al., 2021). Furthermore, since learning attitude (i.e., reading books) may be genetically inherited (Krapohl et al., 2014; Olson, Vernon, Harris, & Jang, 2001), it adds to the genetic propensity of crystallized intelligence. Taken together, these empirical findings including ours may challenge the historical conceptualization that fluid intelligence may be more driven by genes and crystallized intelligence by the environment (Cattell, 1971).

Our structural equation models show the potential relationships among the genes, brain sex, and cognition. The results indicate that a higher brain maleness score (a lower femaleness score) positively modulates the positive effect of the cognitive GPS on general intelligence significantly in both sexes. Considering that the modulatory effect remains significant after controlling for several potential confounding factors of the brain and cognitive performance, this GPS-brain sex-intelligence pathway has a significant statistical association. These results thus suggest the novel role of brain sex in children, linking the genetic influence to cognitive performance.

Then, what is the biological account of the modulatory effects of the brain sex on the genetic influence on cognitive performance: that is, positive toward maleness and negative toward femaleness? Literature shows that sex chromosomes play a crucial role in cognitive performance (Bender, Puck, Salbenblatt, & Robinson, 1990; Hong & Reiss, 2014; Warling et al., 2020). However, since we did not include the sex chromosomes when constructing the GPSs (following the common practice of the GWAS designs to boost statistical power), it might not fully explain the differences in the mediation effects across sex. Alternatively, we speculate that the different expression patterns of autosomal variants across sex (Boraska et al., 2012; Wijchers & Festenstein, 2011; Zuo et al., 2015) may account for the modulatory effects of sex. Indeed, in line with this speculation, recent literature highlights sex differences in brain transcriptomes related to schizophrenia and alcohol effects (Hitzemann et al., 2021; Hoffman et al., 2022). Future research may test the association between sex differences in genetic expression in the brain and neurocognitive development.

Note that only females showed a significant correlation between brain-based sex score and cognitive GPSs, whereas males showed a marginally significant correlation after correction for multiple comparisons. We think this should not be interpreted as the female-only effect of the cognitive GPSs in the brain sex development. Rather, it should be noted that their effect sizes were similar across sex (in educational attainment GPS) and the models combining males and females showed the significant correlations of the brain-based sex score and cognitive GPSs (in educational attainment and cognitive performance GPSs). Furthermore, regardless of the modulatory effects of sex, in both females and males, the influence of the cognitive GPSs on cognition was positive. This is in line with the literature in adults (Lee et al., 2018; Savage et al., 2018). Taken together, we think that the genetic underpinnings of cognitive development might be related to sex differentiation in the brain. Therefore, our integrative analysis reveals the subtle relationships among sex, genes, brains, and cognition, otherwise undetectable. We suggest this is a novel biological pathway to individual differences in brain sex. It may be interesting to test whether this pathway is related to epigenetic effects of environmental factors, such as early life stress.

This study confirms that biological sex can be classified accurately based on morphometric and white matter connectivity. A recent study with ABCD data show that the biological sex was classified with 89.6% accuracy in the replication set using a deep neural network trained on ABCD T1-weighted structural MRI (Adeli et al., 2020). Our study extends this prior work by showing the additive classification performance increase with the diffusion white matter connectomes. This performance increase perhaps presents that the multimodal MRI effectively accounts for the heterogeneous developmental trajectories of grey and white matter (Giedd et al., 1999). It further shows the importance of the multimodal MRI approach in accurate delineation of brain development status.

Our brain features exclude the total volumes of the brain, grey and white matter, of which the sex differences have been reported (Ruigrok et al., 2014). Though the whole brain volume difference between sexes may be a biological aspect, we reasoned that the measures of gross anatomy would confound the brain–cognition relationship. Therefore, beyond the sex difference in the gross anatomy, this study shows that the patterns of the grey matter and white matter fibers are associated with the continuum of brain sex.

In testing the relationships among the brain sex, cognitive ability, and the genetic influence on cognitive ability, we focused on the cognitive GPSs. However, our discovery of the significant tripartite correlation among the brain-based sex score, total brain volume, and intelligence may lead to a question whether the genetic underpinning of cognitive ability is related to that of the total brain size. Indeed, a recent GWAS meta-analysis reveals an overlap of GWAS hits between cognitive intelligence and brain size in 5 genomic loci (Jansen et al., 2020). We hope that future research may test the moderation effect of sex on the genetic influence on brain size and its impact on cognitive intelligence.

In our study, we found no significant relationship among our key variables with salivary measures of sex hormones. Given the prepubertal stages of the participants, the negative statistical findings may reflect that the gene–brain sex–cognition relationship is not significantly related to the effects of sex hormones. Literature shows a complex relationship between the level of sex hormones and cognitive intelligence (Castanho et al., 2014; Gurvich, Hoy, Thomas, & Kulkarni, 2018). Though different sex hormonal levels across the sexes are observed from the prepubertal ages (Courant et al., 2010), the actual effect of the sex hormones on cognitive intelligence (or its modulation) may not appear until puberty (Shangguan & Shi, 2009).

This study shows a novel relationship among genetic factors, brain sex, and cognitive intelligence. The link between genome-wide factors and cognitive ability has been shown in previous studies. Cognitive GPSs account for general cognitive ability up to 3.5% in pre-adolescence children (Allegrini et al., 2019), 11% of the variance in general intelligence, and 16% of the variance in educational achievement in adolescents (Selzam et al., 2017). Extending this literature, our study shows that an individual's degree of brain sex may modulate the impact of the genetic factor on cognitive intelligence. Since this modulatory effect is positive toward brain maleness and negative toward brain femaleness, it adds another source of sex and individual variability in intelligence. This inference also presents the benefit of using the brain data as an endophenotype in assessing the genotype–phenotype association (Glahn, Thompson, & Blangero, 2007). Taken together, brain sex is linked to the inherited genetic influence of cognition, accounting for a novel pathway to the individual difference in cognitive intelligence in preadolescence.

In contrast to the multiethnic participants, the SEM in the European-ancestry participants only showed nonsignificant indirect effects of brain sex score. The discrepant results may not be easily reconciled. It should be noted that the cross-ethnic transferability of our cognitive GPS based on the European-ancestry GWAS remains to be validated. However, our cognitive GPS was rigorously adjusted for the potential ethnic confounding. Our result of the significant modulatory effects in the admixed American participants needs to be interpreted with caution.

This study shows the novel relationships among brain sex, cognition, and cognitive GPSs. The brain sex score based on grey matter morphometric and white matter connectivity may represent a neurodevelopmental process in preadolescence related to the inherited genetic influence on cognitive intelligence and unrelated to sex hormonal levels. This study thus provides a novel framework for future research in neurocognitive development and mental disorders.

No comments:

Post a Comment